全文获取类型
收费全文 | 106篇 |
免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
108篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 1篇 |
2020年 | 1篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 3篇 |
2017年 | 5篇 |
2016年 | 3篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 3篇 |
2013年 | 11篇 |
2012年 | 6篇 |
2011年 | 6篇 |
2009年 | 1篇 |
2008年 | 7篇 |
2007年 | 3篇 |
2006年 | 5篇 |
2005年 | 4篇 |
2004年 | 2篇 |
2003年 | 3篇 |
2002年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
1959年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有108条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
The trap-tube problem is used to assess whether an individual is able to foresee the outcome of its actions. To solve the
task, an animal must use a tool to push a piece of food out of a tube, which has a trap along its length. An animal may learn
to avoid the trap through a rule based on associative processes, e.g. using the distance of trap or food as a cue, or by understanding
relations between cause and effect. This task has been used to test physical cognition in a number of tool-using species,
but never a non-tool-user. We developed an experimental design that enabled us to test non-tool-using rooks, Corvus frugilegus. Our modification of the task removed the cognitive requirements of active tool use but still allowed us to test whether
rooks can solve the trap-tube problem, and if so how. Additionally, we developed two new control tasks to determine whether
rooks were able to transfer knowledge to similar, but novel problems, thus revealing more about the mechanisms involved in
solving the task. We found that three out of seven rooks solved the modified trap-tube problem task, showing that the ability
to solve the trap-tube problem is not restricted to tool-using animals. We found no evidence that the birds solved the task
using an understanding of its causal properties, given that none of the birds passed the novel transfer tasks. 相似文献
52.
Susan J. Curry Robin J. Mermelstein Sherry L. Emery Amy K. Sporer Michael L. Berbaum Richard T. Campbell Brian Flay Richard B. Warnecke 《American journal of community psychology》2013,51(1-2):15-29
Most youth cessation treatment research consists of efficacy studies in which treatments are evaluated under optimal conditions of delivery. Less is known about the effectiveness of youth cessation treatments delivered in real-world, community based settings. A national sample of 41 community-based youth cessation programs participated in a longitudinal evaluation to identify site, program, and participant characteristics associated with successful cessation. Validated quit rates were comparable to those in randomized controlled trials; 7-day abstinence at the end of program averaged 14% and 30-day abstinence at 12 months averaged 12%. Multivariate GEE models explored predictors of smoking cessation at the end of the programs and at 12 months. Results showed correlates of both short- and long-term cessation. Findings point to the importance of both individual and community-level variables, including motivation, opportunities for and encouragement to engage in activities outside of academics, having youth participate in treatment before they become highly dependent smokers, and community norms and ordinances that discourage youth purchase, use and possession of tobacco. Providing evidence-based treatment to youth in community-based settings results in successful cessation. 相似文献
53.
Nancy L. McElwain Ashley S. Holland Jennifer M. Engle Maria S. Wong Helen T. Emery 《Infant and child development》2015,24(4):414-434
Moderate challenges provide opportunities for young children to develop coping skills, yet little is known about factors related to individual differences in early coping. This study investigated child–mother attachment security (9‐point rating) assessed via a modified Strange Situation procedure, child expressive language, and child social fearfulness as contributors to coping responses among 33‐month‐old children (N = 128, 66 girls) following receipt of an empty gift box. Associations between child–mother attachment security and child coping were moderated by child characteristics. As hypothesized, greater attachment security was associated with more action towards the stressor (i.e. vocalizing about the empty box and searching for the missing gift), but only for children high on social fearfulness. Further, greater attachment security was associated with seeking more physical proximity to mothers for children low on expressive language and less physical proximity for children high on expressive language. Lastly, greater attachment security was related to more self‐distraction for children with moderate to high expressive language. Results are discussed in terms of child–mother attachment security as a psychological resource constructed over time, which the child may draw upon when confronted with a moderate non‐attachment‐related stressor. Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
54.
Melanie Wakefield George I. Balch Erin Ruel Yvonne Terry‐McElrath Glen Szczypka Brian Flay Sherry Emery Katherine Clegg‐Smith 《Journal of applied social psychology》2005,35(9):1894-1910
Emotional reactions to anti‐smoking advertising (e.g., fear, sadness, anger) may play an important role in promoting smoking‐related attitudinal and behavioral change. Overall, 278 youth completed response ratings of 16 different elements of 50 anti‐smoking ads made by tobacco‐control agencies, tobacco companies, and pharmaceutical companies. Compared with tobacco‐control ads, tobacco‐company ads were more likely to elicit positive emotions and less likely to elicit negative emotions and to be of interest to youth. Compared with tobacco‐control ads, pharmaceutical company ads were less likely to elicit negative emotional responses or cognitively engage youth and more likely to elicit positive emotions. These findings suggest that youth may be unlikely to respond to tobacco‐company advertising in ways that may lead to a lower likelihood of smoking. 相似文献
55.
MacLean EL Matthews LJ Hare BA Nunn CL Anderson RC Aureli F Brannon EM Call J Drea CM Emery NJ Haun DB Herrmann E Jacobs LF Platt ML Rosati AG Sandel AA Schroepfer KK Seed AM Tan J van Schaik CP Wobber V 《Animal cognition》2012,15(2):223-238
Now more than ever animal studies have the potential to test hypotheses regarding how cognition evolves. Comparative psychologists have developed new techniques to probe the cognitive mechanisms underlying animal behavior, and they have become increasingly skillful at adapting methodologies to test multiple species. Meanwhile, evolutionary biologists have generated quantitative approaches to investigate the phylogenetic distribution and function of phenotypic traits, including cognition. In particular, phylogenetic methods can quantitatively (1) test whether specific cognitive abilities are correlated with life history (e.g., lifespan), morphology (e.g., brain size), or socio-ecological variables (e.g., social system), (2) measure how strongly phylogenetic relatedness predicts the distribution of cognitive skills across species, and (3) estimate the ancestral state of a given cognitive trait using measures of cognitive performance from extant species. Phylogenetic methods can also be used to guide the selection of species comparisons that offer the strongest tests of a priori predictions of cognitive evolutionary hypotheses (i.e., phylogenetic targeting). Here, we explain how an integration of comparative psychology and evolutionary biology will answer a host of questions regarding the phylogenetic distribution and history of cognitive traits, as well as the evolutionary processes that drove their evolution. 相似文献
56.
57.
Olmsted Allison E. Markham Christine M. Shegog Ross Ugueto Ana M. Johnson Erica L. Peskin Melissa F. Emery Susan T. Baker Kimberley A. Newlin Elizabeth W. 《Journal of child and family studies》2022,31(7):2050-2064
Journal of Child and Family Studies - Mental illness in adolescence is associated with high-risk sexual behaviors including multiple sex partners, infrequent or inconsistent condom use, and nonuse... 相似文献
58.
It has been hypothesized that older adults are especially susceptible to proactive interference (PI) and that this may contribute to age differences in working memory performance. In young adults, individual differences in PI affect both working memory and reasoning ability, but the relations between PI, working memory, and reasoning in older adults have not been examined. In the current study, young, old, and very old adults performed a modified operation span task that induced several cycles of PI buildup and release as well as two tests of abstract reasoning ability. Age differences in working memory scores increased as PI built up, consistent with the hypothesis that older adults are more susceptible to PI, but both young and older adults showed complete release from PI. Young adults' reasoning ability was best predicted by working memory performance under high PI conditions, replicating M. Bunting (2006). In contrast, older adults' reasoning ability was best predicted by their working memory performance under low PI conditions, thereby raising questions regarding the general role of susceptibility to PI in differences in higher cognitive function among older adults. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2008 APA, all rights reserved). 相似文献
59.
Andre Almeida Emery Schubert John Smith Joe Wolfe 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2017,79(7):1892-1896
Brightness is an attribute often used by musicians when describing timbral characteristics. It is related to the spectral distribution of energy, as is sharpness, studied by Zwicker (Psychoacoustics: Facts and Models, 1990). In the current work, subjects adjusted the spectral slope and thus the spectral centroid (SC) of one of a pair of sounds to make it twice as bright as the other, so as to build a perceptual scale. The ratio of SC required to double brightness is a little less than 2 and decreases as the SC of the tones increases. For these tones, the ratio of brightness is statistically different from the ratio of sharpness calculated from published models. 相似文献
60.