全文获取类型
收费全文 | 201篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
204篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 5篇 |
2019年 | 4篇 |
2018年 | 8篇 |
2017年 | 9篇 |
2016年 | 11篇 |
2015年 | 2篇 |
2014年 | 5篇 |
2013年 | 40篇 |
2012年 | 5篇 |
2011年 | 6篇 |
2010年 | 2篇 |
2009年 | 4篇 |
2008年 | 1篇 |
2007年 | 5篇 |
2006年 | 5篇 |
2005年 | 7篇 |
2004年 | 11篇 |
2003年 | 7篇 |
2002年 | 5篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1964年 | 1篇 |
1962年 | 1篇 |
1959年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有204条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
82.
The Gain-Loss model is a probabilistic skill multimap model for assessing learning processes. In practical applications, more than one skill multimap could be plausible, while none corresponds to the true one. The article investigates whether constraining the error probabilities is a way of uncovering the best skill assignment among a number of alternatives. A simulation study shows that this approach allows the detection of the models that are closest to the correct one. An empirical application shows that it allows the detection of models that are entirely derived from plausible assumptions about the skills required for solving the problems. 相似文献
83.
Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were elicited by words in a free recall paradigm that included a novel item. The P300 component of the ERP is elicited by novel, task-relevant events, and we tested the hypothesis that P300 is a manifestation of the cognitive processing invoked during “context updating.” If the degree to which current representations in working memory need revision is related to P300 amplitude, then the P300 elicited by a given item should be related to the ability to recall that item on a subsequent test. Forty lists were presented to 12 subjects in each of two sessions. The lists were 15 words long, and 1 word, in position 6 through 10, was “isolated” by changing its size. Most subjects recalled these isolated words more often than other words in the same positions (von Restorff effect), and these words also elicited larger P300s than other words. Analysis of variance on the component scores from a principal components analysis revealed that words recalled had a larger amplitude P300 (on initial presentation) than words not recalled. Striking individual differences emerged, and there were strong relationships between the von Restorff effect, overall recall performance, mnemonic strategies, and the association between components of the ERP and recall performance. The overall recall performance of subjects who reported simple (rote) mnemonic strategies was low, but they showed a high von Restorff effect. For these subjects the amplitude of the P300 elicited by words during initial presentation predicted later recall. In contrast, subjects who reported complex mnemonic strategies remembered a high percentage of words and did not show a von Restorff effect. For these subjects P300 did not predict later recall, although a later “slow wave” component of the ERP did. The initial response to isolated items was the same for all subjects (a large P300), and all subjects recognized the isolates faster than other words in a recognition test given at the end of each session. The subjects in whom P300 did not predict recall reported mnemonic strategies that involved organizing the material. These strategies continue long after the time period reflected by P300 (600 msec). Because they were so effective they may have overshadowed the relationship between P300 and recall, which is based on the initial encoding of an event. Our interpretations were further confirmed and clarified from data obtained in a final grand recall and in the recognition test. 相似文献
84.
Roger E. Beaty Paul J. Silvia Emily C. Nusbaum Emanuel Jauk Mathias Benedek 《Memory & cognition》2014,42(7):1186-1197
How does the mind produce creative ideas? Past research has pointed to important roles of both executive and associative processes in creative cognition. But such work has largely focused on the influence of one ability or the other—executive or associative—so the extent to which both abilities may jointly affect creative thought remains unclear. Using multivariate structural equation modeling, we conducted two studies to determine the relative influences of executive and associative processes in domain-general creative cognition (i.e., divergent thinking). Participants completed a series of verbal fluency tasks, and their responses were analyzed by means of latent semantic analysis (LSA) and scored for semantic distance as a measure of associative ability. Participants also completed several measures of executive function—including broad retrieval ability (Gr) and fluid intelligence (Gf). Across both studies, we found substantial effects of both associative and executive abilities: As the average semantic distance between verbal fluency responses and cues increased, so did the creative quality of divergent-thinking responses (Study 1 and Study 2). Moreover, the creative quality of divergent-thinking responses was predicted by the executive variables—Gr (Study 1) and Gf (Study 2). Importantly, the effects of semantic distance and the executive function variables remained robust in the same structural equation model predicting divergent thinking, suggesting unique contributions of both constructs. The present research extends recent applications of LSA in creativity research and provides support for the notion that both associative and executive processes underlie the production of novel ideas. 相似文献
85.
Identification of the second of two brief targets is impaired at intertarget lags of less than about 500 msec. We compared
two accounts of thisattentional blink (AB) by manipulating the number of digit distractors—and hence the lag—inserted among three letter targets in a rapid serial
visual presentation stream of digit distractors. On the resource-depletion hypothesis, longer lags provide more time for processing
the leading target, thus releasing resources for the trailing target. On the temporary-loss-of-control (TLC) hypothesis, intervening
distractors disrupt the current attentional set, producing a trailing-target deficit. Identification accuracy for trailing
targets was unimpaired not only at lag 1 (conventional lag 1 sparing) but also at later lags, if preceded by another target.
The results supported the TLC hypothesis but not the resource-depletion hypothesis. We conclude that the AB is caused by a
disruption in attentional set when a distractor is presented while the central executive is busy processing a leading target. 相似文献
86.
Látos Melinda Lázár György Ondrik Zoltán Szederkényi Edit Hódi Zoltán Horváth Zoltán Csabai Márta 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》2022,52(1):35-44
Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy - There is an increasing body of evidence proving that transplanted patients’ perceptions of their disease and their emotional state may have an... 相似文献
87.
Emanuel Donchin 《Behavior research methods》1995,27(2):217-223
From 1983 through 1986, several laboratories investigated the role of practice in skill acquisition in a joint project called theLearning Strategies Program. The principal research tool for this project was a video game calledSpace Fortress. The present article summarizes the major findings obtained with Space Fortress, and examines several problems that arise in choosing a video game for use as a research tool. 相似文献
88.
Self psychology,intersubjectivity, and group psychotherapy 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Emanuel Shapiro Ph.D. 《Group》1990,14(3):177-182
The basic concepts of self psychology are presented. The three self-object transferences of mirroring, idealizing, and twinship are described and applied to group therapy. The therapist's role is: 1) to accept the patient's need to idealize the leader, the group, or the specific members; 2) to receive recognition and admiration; and 3) to understand the patients search for twinship. The group therapist also has the responsibility of teaching group members to have empathy for one another. 相似文献
89.
Matthew J. Sharps Megan Herrera Laurel Dunn Emanuel Alcala 《Journal of Investigative Psychology & Offender Profiling》2012,9(2):149-160
Repeated recall, as a result of repeated questioning, is typical of situations involving eyewitness evidence. The present study addressed questions of eyewitness performance with reference to repeated questioning in an initial interview, in a format based on actual police procedures. This experiment focused on eyewitness accuracy, eyewitness confidence, and the addition of false details to eyewitness accounts. Ninety‐two adult respondents were asked to recall all the information they could from a single viewing of a scene depicting an assailant aiming a handgun at a victim. This initial question was followed by three additional questions, in which respondents were asked to report any additional details they could recall. On average, respondents provided several times as many correct as false details to the initial question. However, performance deteriorated significantly to the three subsequent questions; on average, across the three subsequent questions, witnesses recalled nearly as much false as accurate information. Witness confidence was positively related to amount of accurate information recalled. However, confidence was also positively related to the number of instances of erroneous recall. These results indicate that reconfigurative dynamics begin to operate, producing confabulated responses in response to questioning demands, as early as the initial interview. This work may also help to clarify the critical relationship between accuracy and confidence in eyewitness reportage, at least within the framework reflected by the present research. Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
90.
Emanuel Berman Ph.D. 《Psychoanalytic Inquiry》2013,33(2):124-129
This article explores several examples of bullying in psychoanalytic training (often candidates bullied by faculty, but at times faculty bullied by candidates, or mutual bullying among candidates) and within psychoanalytic societies. It proposes that this may happen both in homogeneous groups (where individuals seen as disloyal to the common views may be harassed) and in heterogeneous groups (where theoretical disagreements and competition may deteriorate). Interpretations outside the consulting room (and in some instances even within an analytic setting) may become a subtle form of bullying. Although many professional and political groups may exhibit subtle and rationalized forms of bullying, their impact is more powerful in psychoanalytic groups on the background of transference–countertransference feelings originating in members' personal analyses with other members. A readiness to expose and discuss such episodes is crucial if we wish to reduce their destructive influence in training and in subsequent relations among analytic colleagues. 相似文献