首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   80351篇
  免费   4190篇
  国内免费   40篇
  2020年   903篇
  2019年   1389篇
  2018年   1695篇
  2017年   1867篇
  2016年   1886篇
  2015年   1372篇
  2014年   1580篇
  2013年   6987篇
  2012年   2547篇
  2011年   2745篇
  2010年   1778篇
  2009年   1753篇
  2008年   2508篇
  2007年   2356篇
  2006年   2109篇
  2005年   1895篇
  2004年   1780篇
  2003年   1736篇
  2002年   1739篇
  2001年   2659篇
  2000年   2570篇
  1999年   1893篇
  1998年   966篇
  1997年   824篇
  1996年   743篇
  1995年   755篇
  1994年   756篇
  1993年   752篇
  1992年   1567篇
  1991年   1498篇
  1990年   1436篇
  1989年   1321篇
  1988年   1262篇
  1987年   1270篇
  1986年   1229篇
  1985年   1349篇
  1984年   1055篇
  1983年   874篇
  1979年   1047篇
  1978年   786篇
  1976年   707篇
  1975年   930篇
  1974年   971篇
  1973年   1006篇
  1972年   867篇
  1971年   744篇
  1969年   717篇
  1968年   868篇
  1967年   817篇
  1966年   802篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
971.
Forty-eight American children, 12 popular and 12 rejected children from Grades 3 and 6, were paired with same-sex friends and nonfriends on an interactive task. Children were videotaped and their interactions judged for the amount and type of attention each child in the dyad gave to each other and the conversation. Main dependent measures included mutual engagements, acknowledgement of the partner, getting the attention of the partner, attention to the conversation, and social impact of the utterances. Both rejected and popular children attended to the conversation and the partner, but rejected children appeared to overattend in several ways. The interactions of rejected children and their matches involved more mutual engagements, conversational initiators, facilitators, terminators, and nonverbal attention-getting devices. Sex and developmental effects were also found.  相似文献   
972.
973.
974.
The time course of phonological code activation in two writing systems   总被引:18,自引:0,他引:18  
  相似文献   
975.
976.
Four experiments examined the memory processes used to maintain location in a counting sequence. In Experiment 1, subjects who rapidly counted forward omitted many repeated-digit numbers (e.g., 77), as found previously with backward counting. Subjects in Experiment 2 counted backward with normal auditory feedback or with headphones through which white noise was channeled. In both cases, repeated-digit errors predominated, suggesting that the contents of short-term memory, rather than auditory sensory memory, are checked during counting. In Experiment 3, subjects silently wrote counting responses, and the omission errors resembled those in vocal counting. Repetition errors were also found and attributed to phonological recoding failures. Articulatory suppression in Experiment 4 greatly increased the number of repetition errors in the written counting task. A model of the counting process was proposed according to which subjects keep track of their location in the counting sequence by monitoring phonologically coded short-term memory representations of the numbers.  相似文献   
977.
978.
This experiment compared the effects of high-level and low-level postpassage questions, when presented immediately after the passage segment containing the answer to the question, on college students' free recall of expository prose passages. Low-vocabulary subjects' recall of high-level information not specifically quizzed by the postpassage questions was significantly greater in the condition in which the questions quizzed a high-level unit than in the low-question and no-question conditions, p less than .05. These results are interpreted as being consistent with a direct-access explanation which assumes that questions presented in close temporal contiguity to the quizzed information directly access that information in memory and that fewer associative links have to be traversed in the spread of activation from high-level units to other related information in the hierarchy. The effect of type of question in this experiment was compared with that found in an earlier experiment (Wilhite, 1982) involving delayed question presentation.  相似文献   
979.
An assessment of the Hasher and Zacks (1979) conceptualization of the automatic nature of the acquisition of spatial location information was made in the context of manipulations of subjects' intention to learn and mood state. The Velten (1968) mood induction procedure was used to establish depressed and nondepressed subjects. Half of each group was then presented with a matrix of geometric forms under instructions to learn the location of the forms (intentional) or to evaluate the pleasantness of the array (incidental). Subsequent tests of spatial location performance found no differences as a function of subjects' mood state, but did find a strong effect for instructional condition. The superior performance of the intentional subjects raises questions about the automatic nature of the encoding of spatial location.  相似文献   
980.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号