全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1389篇 |
免费 | 90篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1480篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 21篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 28篇 |
2020年 | 35篇 |
2019年 | 23篇 |
2018年 | 52篇 |
2017年 | 57篇 |
2016年 | 56篇 |
2015年 | 44篇 |
2014年 | 57篇 |
2013年 | 277篇 |
2012年 | 111篇 |
2011年 | 86篇 |
2010年 | 52篇 |
2009年 | 44篇 |
2008年 | 90篇 |
2007年 | 89篇 |
2006年 | 65篇 |
2005年 | 38篇 |
2004年 | 23篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 29篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 22篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1946年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有1480条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
The aim of this study was to evaluate a procedure to reveal the emergence of full naming in typically developing children. In Experiment 1, five 6-year-old children (a) learned tacts of pictures and the emergence of the selection of these pictures upon hearing their names was tested; and (b) the selection of other pictures was taught and the emergence of the tacts was tested. All children showed the emergence of picture selection and 3 children showed the emergence of the tacts. In Experiment 2a, the children's correct repetition of the names of 3 pictures was verified first, then the experimenter presented the pictures with their names without requiring any behavior other than attending. Finally, the emergence of picture selection and the tacts was tested. Experiment 2b replicated Experiment 2a with words that were easier to pronounce. Picture selection and the tacts emerged; this performance documented full naming for the first time with typically developing children. These procedures can be used to induce naming capacity in children who lack it; hence, they can be applied to teach children with learning delays. 相似文献
102.
Moji Aghajani Ilya M. Veer Marie-José van Tol André Aleman Mark A. van Buchem Dick J. Veltman Serge A. R. B. Rombouts Nic J. van der Wee 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2014,14(2):836-848
The personality traits neuroticism and extraversion are differentially related to socioemotional functioning and susceptibility to affective disorders. However, the neurobiology underlying this differential relationship is still poorly understood. This discrepancy could perhaps best be studied by adopting a brain connectivity approach. Whereas the amygdala has repeatedly been linked to neuroticism and extraversion, no study has yet focused on the intrinsic functional architecture of amygdala-centered networks in relation to both traits. To this end, seed-based correlation analysis was employed to reveal amygdala resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) and its associations with neuroticism and extraversion in 50 healthy participants. Higher neuroticism scores were associated with increased amygdala RSFC with the precuneus, and decreased amygdala RSFC with the temporal poles, insula, and superior temporal gyrus (p < .05, cluster corrected). Conversely, higher extraversion scores were associated with increased amygdala RSFC with the putamen, temporal pole, insula, and several regions of the occipital cortex (p < .05, cluster corrected). The shifts in amygdala RSFC associated with neuroticism may relate to the less-adaptive perception and processing of self-relevant and socioemotional information that is frequently seen in neurotic individuals, whereas the amygdala RSFC pattern associated with extraversion may relate to the heightened reward sensitivity and enhanced socioemotional functioning in extraverts. We hypothesize that the variability in amygdala RSFC observed in the present study could potentially link neuroticism and extraversion to the neurobiology underlying increased susceptibility or resilience to affective disorders. 相似文献
103.
Imitation,Impersonation, and Transformation: Using Male Role Models in Films to Promote Maturity 下载免费PDF全文
Becoming a man is developmental. Boys model after men in their lives as well as figures they see in the media. When role models are positive, appropriate, and present, boys grow into constructive adults who contribute to society. This article examines how counselors can intentionally help young men mature into well‐grounded and prosocial individuals through the use of models in films. It provides a list of some movies that counselors can use in the process. 相似文献
104.
105.
Framing effects on emotional responses in a green electricity choice are investigated in a survey of a random sample of 655 Swedish residents. The results show that overall more positive and less negative emotions are experienced for a promotion frame (“choosing green electricity and paying more”) than for a prevention frame (“not choosing green electricity and paying less”). Value orientation is shown to moderate the framing effect in that for the promotion frame, individuals with a self‐transcendence value orientation show the most positive and least negative emotion, whereas for the prevention frame, individuals with a self‐enhancement value orientation show the most positive and least negative emotion. Emotional responses are further shown to mediate the relationship between value orientation and intention to purchase eco‐labeled electricity. 相似文献
106.
107.
108.
Serniclaes W Van Heghe S Mousty P Carré R Sprenger-Charolles L 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2004,87(4):336-361
Perceptual discrimination between speech sounds belonging to different phoneme categories is better than that between sounds falling within the same category. This property, known as "categorical perception," is weaker in children affected by dyslexia. Categorical perception develops from the predispositions of newborns for discriminating all potential phoneme categories in the world's languages. Predispositions that are not relevant for phoneme perception in the ambient language are usually deactivated during early childhood. However, the current study shows that dyslexic children maintain a higher sensitivity to phonemic distinctions irrelevant in their linguistic environment. This suggests that dyslexic children use an allophonic mode of speech perception that, although without straightforward consequences for oral communication, has obvious implications for the acquisition of alphabetic writing. Allophonic perception specifically affects the mapping between graphemes and phonemes, contrary to other manifestations of dyslexia, and may be a core deficit. 相似文献
109.
In this article, normative data on the familiarity and difficulty of 196 single-solution Spanish word fragments are presented.
The database includes the following indices: difficulty, familiarity, frequency, number of meanings, number of letters given
in the fragment, first and/or last letters given, and ratio of letters to blanks. A factor analysis was performed on difficulty,
and two factors were obtained. Frequency, familiarity, and number of meanings loaded highly on the first factor, which we
consider to measure lexical processes, whereas number of letters in the fragment, first and/or last letters given, and ratio
of letters to blanks loaded highly on the second factor, which we judge to be determined by perceptual information. Regression
analyses using factor scores as predictors showed that both factors accounted for a significant part of the completion probability
scores. The full set of these norms may be downloaded from the Psychonomic Society Web archive atwww.psychonomic.org/archive/. 相似文献
110.
Two self-paced reading experiments investigated syntactic ambiguity resolution in Spanish. The experiments examined the way in which Spanish subjects initially interpret sentences that are temporarily ambiguous between a sentence complement and a relative clause interpretation. Experiment 1 examined whether the sentence complement preference found in English is observed in Spanish speaking subjects. In Experiment 2, verbal mood was manipulated in order to study the influence of verb-specific information on sentence processing. Since subcategorization for a subjunctive complement clause is generally assumed to be a lexical property of some verbs, the manipulation of the mood of the embedded verb affords us an interesting and novel way to examine the influence of lexical information on syntactic ambiguity resolution. Experiment 1 showed that Spanish speakers initially interpret the ambiguous that-clause as a sentence complement. Experiment 2 showed that verb-specific information, in particular, the information that specificies that a verb subcategorizes for a subjunctive complement, is accessed and used rapidly and affects the ambiguity resolution process. The results are discussed in relation to current models of sentence processing. 相似文献