首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   797篇
  免费   23篇
  2022年   9篇
  2021年   8篇
  2020年   11篇
  2019年   23篇
  2018年   18篇
  2017年   21篇
  2016年   26篇
  2015年   14篇
  2014年   24篇
  2013年   83篇
  2012年   43篇
  2011年   29篇
  2010年   24篇
  2009年   24篇
  2008年   23篇
  2007年   25篇
  2006年   23篇
  2005年   32篇
  2004年   18篇
  2003年   25篇
  2002年   22篇
  2001年   22篇
  2000年   25篇
  1999年   21篇
  1998年   8篇
  1997年   9篇
  1996年   14篇
  1994年   6篇
  1992年   11篇
  1991年   7篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   11篇
  1988年   7篇
  1987年   12篇
  1985年   7篇
  1984年   9篇
  1983年   10篇
  1982年   7篇
  1981年   5篇
  1979年   7篇
  1978年   6篇
  1977年   4篇
  1976年   7篇
  1975年   5篇
  1973年   4篇
  1972年   6篇
  1971年   6篇
  1969年   6篇
  1966年   7篇
  1920年   5篇
排序方式: 共有820条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
801.
One early‐developing component of theory of mind is an understanding of the link between sensory perception and knowledge formation. We know little about the extent to which children's first‐hand sensory experiences drive the development of this understanding, as most tasks capturing this early understanding target vision, with less attention paid to the other senses. In this study, 64 typically hearing children (Mage = 4.0 years) and 21 orally educated deaf children (Mage = 5.44 years) were asked to identify which of two informants knew the identity of a toy animal when each had differing perceptual access to the animal. In the ‘seeing’ condition, one informant saw the animal and the other did not; in the ‘hearing’ condition, one informant heard the animal and the other did not. For both hearing and deaf children, there was no difference between performance on hearing and seeing trials, but deaf children were delayed in both conditions. Further, within both the hearing and deaf groups, older children outperformed younger children on these tasks, indicating that there is a developmental progression. Taken together, the pattern of results suggests that experiences other than first‐hand sensory experiences drive children's developing understanding that sensory perception is associated with knowledge.  相似文献   
802.
ABSTRACT

Alcohol use is associated with poorer smoking cessation-related outcomes, and smokers with elevated levels of worry experience greater smoking cessation problems. Yet, little is known about the explanatory mechanisms that may underlie the relationship between trait worry and hazardous drinking among smokers. Therefore, this study explored the explanatory roles of coping and conformity drinking motives in the relationship between trait worry and hazardous drinking outcomes including alcohol consumption, alcohol problems, maximum number of drinks, and the number of prior alcohol quit attempts among treatment-seeking smokers. Participants included 377 treatment-seeking smokers who consumed at least one alcoholic drink in the last year (48% female; 86.2% Caucasian; M age = 34.83 years, SD = 13.38). Results showed a significant indirect effect of trait worry through coping-related drinking motives in relation to alcohol consumption, alcohol problems, maximum number of drinks, and number of prior drinking quit attempts. These findings were evident after controlling for gender, cigarette dependence, and current psychopathology. These findings suggest that coping drinking motives are one mechanism that may explain the relation between trait worry and hazardous drinking outcomes among treatment-seeking smokers.  相似文献   
803.
Memory for conceptually isolated (distinctive) words was investigated in two experiments. In Experiment 1, recognition of distinctive targets was compared with recognition of targets from homogeneous lists and with recognition of background information. Distinctive targets were better recognized than the same words presented in homogeneous lists. No effects of distinctiveness on the recognition accuracy of background items were observed. These results fail to support the hypothesis that distinctive information receives extra resources during encoding at the expense of surrounding background information. In Experiment 2 the effects of distinctiveness on recall were evaluated. Distinctive targets were more likely to be recalled than targets from homogeneous lists. However, unlike the effects found in recognition, background items were more poorly recalled from lists containing distinctive targets than from homogeneous lists. Organizational processes in recall were also evaluated. There was greater subjective organization for target and background items from lists containing distinctive targets than from lists containing nondistinctive targets. These results were discussed in terms of encoding and retrieval explanations of the effects of distinctiveness.  相似文献   
804.
805.
806.
807.
Psychiatric inpatient admission of three nondepressed young men who escaped deadly self-injury provided an opportunity to study their character organization. Defects in affect-regulatory functions and evidences of pathological narcissism were identified and explored. Each patient had a specific suicide-risk consultation and a psychotherapy evaluation. Each denied intent to kill himself, and none acknowledged experience of depression or the wish to die. Each also denied his suicidal behavior involved significant risks, and each discounted the importance of obvious, identifiable stressors as triggers for it. The interrelation between pathological narcissism and this particular suicidal behavior is discussed. These observations can assist in the assessment of suicide risk in nondepressed patients.  相似文献   
808.
809.
810.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号