全文获取类型
收费全文 | 225篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
236篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 1篇 |
2020年 | 3篇 |
2019年 | 9篇 |
2018年 | 6篇 |
2017年 | 8篇 |
2016年 | 7篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 2篇 |
2013年 | 19篇 |
2012年 | 14篇 |
2011年 | 14篇 |
2010年 | 7篇 |
2009年 | 11篇 |
2008年 | 9篇 |
2007年 | 7篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 6篇 |
2004年 | 11篇 |
2003年 | 9篇 |
2002年 | 7篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1964年 | 1篇 |
1962年 | 1篇 |
1959年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有236条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
Motivational synchronicity: Priming motivational orientations with observations of others’ behaviors
Ron Friedman Edward L. Deci Andrew J. Elliot Arlen C. Moller Henk Aarts 《Motivation and emotion》2010,34(1):34-38
Two experiments tested the motivational synchronicity hypothesis, according to which observation of a target person’s behavior
implying an intrinsic or an extrinsic motivational orientation primes the observers’ corresponding motivational orientation.
Experiment 1 revealed that participants exposed to a target person intrinsically motivated to perform a task, relative to
those exposed to an extrinsically motivated target person, showed greater intrinsic motivation (free-choice persistence) for
the same task. Experiment 2 extended this in two important ways: (1) different tasks were used for the target and participant
in order to rule out an expectation-based interpretation of the results, and (2) performance on an activity known to be facilitated
by intrinsic motivation was used as the dependent measure. It appears that simply observing others’ motivational orientations
influences the accessibility of the observers’ corresponding motivational orientation. 相似文献
92.
93.
Elliot E. Isom Juliana Groves-Radomski Melissa M. McConaha 《Journal of Creativity in Mental Health》2015,10(3):339-350
The application of creative techniques in counseling is often tailored to a unique problem, and limited by individual characteristics of the client. One intervention, sandtray therapy, a derivative of play therapy, has gained strong momentum as a creative technique. With greater flexibility for treating a much larger population, sandtray therapy is more applicable for families than other creative approaches. The purpose of the current article is to give an overview of sand tray therapy, as well as provide information regarding its use for working with families. Specifically, the authors discuss exactly how counselors can successfully incorporate sandtray therapy in the familial population setting, and describe its application through case study. 相似文献
94.
Andrew J. Elliot Nawal Aldhobaiban Kou Murayama Ahmed Kobeisy Małgorzata A. Gocłowska Aber Khyat 《International journal of psychology》2018,53(1):16-22
In this research, we investigate impression management (IM) as a substantive personality variable by linking it to differentiated achievement motivation constructs, namely achievement motives (workmastery, competitiveness, fear of failure) and achievement goals (mastery‐approach, mastery‐avoidance, performance‐approach, performance‐avoidance). Study 1 revealed that IM was a positive predictor of workmastery and a negative predictor of competitiveness (with and without self‐deceptive enhancement (SDE) controlled). Studies 2a and 2b revealed that IM was a positive predictor of mastery‐approach goals and mastery‐avoidance goals (without and, in Study 2b, with SDE controlled). These findings highlight the value of conceptualising and utilising IM as a personality variable in its own right and shed light on the nature of the achievement motive and achievement goal constructs. 相似文献
95.
96.
97.
Maureen D. Satyshur Elliot A. Layden Jennifer R. Gowins Angel Buchanan Jacqueline K. Gollan 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2018,18(5):884-901
Ruminative thinking is related to an increased risk for major depressive disorder (MDD) and perpetuates negative mood states. Rumination, uncontrollable negative thoughts about the self, may comprise both reflective and brooding components. However, only brooding rumination is consistently associated with increased negativity bias and negative coping styles, while reflective rumination has a less clear relationship with negative outcomes in healthy and depressed participants. The current study examined seed-to-voxel (S2.V) resting-state functional connectivity (FC) in a sample of healthy (HC) and depressed (MDD) adult women (HC: n=50, MDD: n=33). The S2V FC of six key brain regions, including the left and right amygdala, anterior and posterior cingulate cortex (ACC, PCC), and medial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices (mPFC, dlPFC), was correlated with self-reported reflective and brooding rumination. Results indicate that HC and MDD participants had increased brooding rumination associated with decreased FC between the left amygdala and the right temporal pole. Moreover, reflective rumination was associated with distinct FC of the mPFC, PCC, and ACC with parietal, occipital, and cingulate regions. Depressed participants, compared with HC, exhibited decreased FC between the PCC and a region in the right middle frontal gyrus. The results of the current study add to the understanding of the neural underpinnings of different forms of self-related cognition—brooding and reflective rumination—in healthy and depressed women. 相似文献
98.
Alinda Friedman Elliot A. Ludvig Eric L. G. Legge Quoc C. Vuong 《Behavior research methods》2013,45(1):98-107
We extend a Bayesian method for combining estimates of means and variances from independent cues in a spatial cue-combination paradigm. In a typical cue-combination experiment, subjects estimate a value on a single dimension—for example, depth—on the basis of two different cues, such as retinal disparity and motion. The mathematics for this one-dimensional case is well established. When the variable to be estimated has two dimensions, such as location (which has both x and y values), then the one-dimensional method may be inappropriate due to possible correlations between x and y and the fact that the dimensions may be inseparable. A cue-combination task for location involves people or animals estimating xy locations under two single-cue conditions and in a condition in which both cues are combined. We present the mathematics for the two-dimensional case in an analogous manner to the one-dimensional case and illustrate them using a numeric example. Our example involves locations on maps, but the method illustrated is relevant for any task for which the estimated variable has two or more dimensions. 相似文献
99.
In two studies, college students’ achievement goals were linked to their perceptions of closeness to their parents following an exam. In Study 1, mastery-approach goals positively predicted perceived closeness immediately after an exam, prior to receiving feedback. In Study 2, mastery-approach goals again positively predicted perceived closeness, but performance–avoidance goals interacted with exam performance in predicting closeness. Specifically, poor exam performance predicted feeling distant from parents among those high in performance–avoidance goals but not among those low in performance–avoidance goals. This suggests that parental attachment may be involved in the achievement strivings of adults. 相似文献
100.
This research examines the hypothesis that an attentional process grounded in avoidance motivation-local relative to global processing-mediates the negative effect of red on intellectual performance. This hypothesis was tested in a series of experiments using two approaches to documenting mediation. Experiment 1 established that the perception of red undermines IQ test performance. Experiments 2a and 2b documented mediation via the experimental causal chain approach, and Experiment 3 documented mediation via the measurement of mediation approach. This represents the first demonstration of a mediational process in the domain of color psychology. A call is made to broaden priming research to include color stimuli. 相似文献