全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2552篇 |
免费 | 37篇 |
专业分类
2589篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 17篇 |
2020年 | 30篇 |
2019年 | 59篇 |
2018年 | 51篇 |
2017年 | 57篇 |
2016年 | 66篇 |
2015年 | 55篇 |
2014年 | 57篇 |
2013年 | 268篇 |
2012年 | 96篇 |
2011年 | 106篇 |
2010年 | 84篇 |
2009年 | 63篇 |
2008年 | 92篇 |
2007年 | 100篇 |
2006年 | 96篇 |
2005年 | 96篇 |
2004年 | 78篇 |
2003年 | 77篇 |
2002年 | 74篇 |
2001年 | 45篇 |
2000年 | 38篇 |
1999年 | 33篇 |
1998年 | 27篇 |
1997年 | 33篇 |
1996年 | 27篇 |
1995年 | 36篇 |
1994年 | 23篇 |
1993年 | 27篇 |
1992年 | 26篇 |
1991年 | 24篇 |
1990年 | 24篇 |
1989年 | 23篇 |
1988年 | 29篇 |
1987年 | 25篇 |
1985年 | 36篇 |
1984年 | 36篇 |
1983年 | 20篇 |
1982年 | 25篇 |
1981年 | 17篇 |
1980年 | 35篇 |
1979年 | 32篇 |
1978年 | 24篇 |
1977年 | 24篇 |
1976年 | 32篇 |
1975年 | 28篇 |
1974年 | 30篇 |
1973年 | 20篇 |
1971年 | 16篇 |
1967年 | 18篇 |
排序方式: 共有2589条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Ellen E. Kossek Shaun M. Pichler Darrell Meece Marguerite E. Barratt 《Journal of Occupational & Organizational Psychology》2008,81(3):369-391
Integrating theory from the family ecological systems and social support literatures with findings from child care research, in this study we develop and test a model relating family, friend, and neighbour (FFN) child care provider characteristics to perceived child care quality (provider reports of caregiving behaviours, mother‐provider caregiving relationship) and maternal well‐being (work‐family conflict, depressive symptoms). Results from phone interviews with 187 FFN providers receiving public subsidies indicated that even after controlling for familial status or household income, caregiver perceptions of higher quality care were associated with higher education levels, greater attachment to child care as a job; and lower provider depressive symptoms. After controlling for familial status, data analysed from a subset of 51 mother‐provider pairs, indicated that mothers using care from providers who reported higher quality parent‐caregiver social relationships reported lower work‐family conflict and depressive symptoms. This study suggests mothers who have providers with whom they have good caregiving interactions may experience positive social support and psychological crossover dynamics associated with mother well‐being. 相似文献
992.
993.
Logan GD 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2005,12(4):647-653
An experiment is reported that measured the time it takes to switch attention from one set of locations to another in response
to a cue that indicates the relevant locations. The experiment compared sequences of trials in which the same locations were
cued in succession with sequences in which different locations were cued in succession in order to separate cue-encoding time
from attention-switching time. Same-location sequences require cue encoding but not attention switching. They were substantially
faster than different-location sequences, which require both cue encoding and attention switching. Formal models were fitted
to time-course functions generated by presenting the cues 0, 100, 200, 300, or 400 msec before the target displays. The model
fits suggest that cue encoding took 67–74 msec and attention switching took 76–101 msec. 相似文献
994.
Two studies examined the impact of witness preparation training with criminal defendants. In the first study, mock criminal defendants testified twice on videotape about minor crimes they were accused of but had not committed. Approximately half underwent witness preparation between their first and second testimony simulations. In the second study, eight public defender clients were videotaped during testimony simulations both before and after being prepared to testify. In both studies, ratings by trained evaluators indicated that witness preparation was associated with increases in the use of several testimony delivery skills, improvements in overall testimony quality, and reductions in apparent guilt. Training was also associated in both studies with an unintended decrease in expressiveness. Ratings from the mock defendant study further suggested that prepared defendants were seen as more credible than unprepared defendants because they used more effective testimony delivery skills, and as less likely to be guilty than unprepared defendants because they were perceived as being more credible. 相似文献
995.
Although mindless evaluations typically accompany social comparisons, they are not necessary, and may be costly. We describe how mindlessness enters the social comparison process at two points. First, during the social comparison both self and other are mindlessly de-contextualized, through (1) biased selection of relevant behaviors, (2) biased selection of criteria along which behaviors are compared, (3) lack of knowledge of intent behind behavior, (4) lack of knowledge about representativeness of behavior, (5) lack of knowledge about typicality of future behavior as moderated by learning, (6) improper understanding of the meaning of behavior, and (7) lack of knowledge about motivations generating the comparisons. Second, the affective results of the social comparison are often mindlessly generalized to the global self, while the breadth and complexity of the network of attributes that constitute to the ‘self’ is ignored. Global-self-evaluative social comparisons forfeit the potential of gaining accurate and usable information about personal attributes. 相似文献
996.
Benjamin M. Wilkowski Michael D. RobinsonRobert D. Gordon Wendy Troop-Gordon 《Journal of research in personality》2007
Previous research has shown that trait anger is associated with biases in attention and interpretation, but the temporal relation between these two types of biases remains unresolved. Indeed, two very different models can be derived from the literature. One model proposes that interpretation biases emerge from earlier biases in attention, whereas the other model proposes that hostile interpretations occur quickly, even prior to the allocation of attention to specific cues. Within the context of integrated visual scenes of ambiguously intended harm, the two models make opposite predictions that can be examined using an eye-tracking methodology. The present study (N = 45) therefore tracked participants’ allocation of attention to hostile and non-hostile cues in ambiguous visual scenes, and found support for the idea that high anger individuals make early hostile interpretations prior to encoding hostile and non-hostiles cues. The data are important in understanding associations between trait anger and cognitive biases. 相似文献
997.
998.
999.
W. W. Gordon 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1966,1(2):125-134
The basic scientific discovery made by Pavlov, namely the differentially associated paired system comprising related positive and negative conditioned signals based upon inborn reflex systems, has been applied to normal, neurotic and psychotic states. A hypothesis is suggested as to the causation of neurotic and psychotic disorder. The differentially associated paired systems based upon the reproductive reflexes have to be reversed at puberty in the course of maturation and this disrupts normal function. This is not incompatible with Freudian ideas where “unconscious incestuous wishes, drives and guilt” are believed to be potent factors in the production of functional disorders. Freudian “life instincts” are equated with normal differentially associated paired systems and “death instincts” are equated with those paired systems in a state of reversal (ultraparadoxical phase). 相似文献
1000.
Gordon G. Bechtel 《Psychometrika》1966,31(1):75-84
Scales constructed from paired-comparison designs under Thurstone's Case V model are discussed in relation to those derived from similarity data by means of the Unilateral Law of Comparative Judgment [3]. Factors inherent in the triadic similarity task are then considered with respect to scale invariance across experimental designs. Illustrative data, although revealing the influence of these factors upon similarity response consistency, indicate the similarity scale to be somewhat robust to their biasing effects.This research was supported in part by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH-04439-05. 相似文献