全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3752篇 |
免费 | 38篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
3791篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 27篇 |
2023年 | 38篇 |
2022年 | 34篇 |
2021年 | 54篇 |
2020年 | 83篇 |
2019年 | 104篇 |
2018年 | 127篇 |
2017年 | 138篇 |
2016年 | 191篇 |
2015年 | 89篇 |
2014年 | 95篇 |
2013年 | 372篇 |
2012年 | 187篇 |
2011年 | 219篇 |
2010年 | 130篇 |
2009年 | 120篇 |
2008年 | 170篇 |
2007年 | 158篇 |
2006年 | 156篇 |
2005年 | 123篇 |
2004年 | 122篇 |
2003年 | 118篇 |
2002年 | 111篇 |
2001年 | 56篇 |
2000年 | 51篇 |
1999年 | 49篇 |
1998年 | 41篇 |
1997年 | 36篇 |
1996年 | 45篇 |
1995年 | 43篇 |
1994年 | 42篇 |
1993年 | 21篇 |
1992年 | 25篇 |
1991年 | 27篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 20篇 |
1988年 | 18篇 |
1987年 | 20篇 |
1986年 | 15篇 |
1985年 | 27篇 |
1984年 | 24篇 |
1983年 | 19篇 |
1982年 | 26篇 |
1981年 | 18篇 |
1980年 | 15篇 |
1979年 | 21篇 |
1977年 | 21篇 |
1976年 | 16篇 |
1975年 | 16篇 |
1974年 | 16篇 |
排序方式: 共有3791条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
981.
Elizabeth M. Cohen 《Current Psychology》2000,19(1):46-56
The relationship between exposure to violence and vulnerability to suicide among adolescents was examined. The topic was initiated
by the rapid increase in adolescent's exposure to violence and the sparse systematic research available on its relationship
to attitudes about life and death. The study's main hypothesis is that high levels of exposure to violence are associated
with high suicide risk according to two conceptual paradigms-Fear of Death and Attraction and Repulsion to Life. Eighty-five
adolescents were administered three self report questionnaires. The questionnaires assessed levels of exposure to violence,
fear of death, and attitudes towards life and death. Those who had been exposed to a high level of violence reported attitudes
synonymous with the profile of an individual at risk for suicide (low fear of death, low repulsion to death, and low attraction
to life; high repulsion to life and high attraction to death). 相似文献
982.
Adolescent males and females (grades 6–12) (n = 552) were presented with nine brief vignettes from the Teen Apperception Scale, describing high-achieving hypothetical female (or male) peers, across nine domains: leadership, academics, athleticism (two), musicianship, art, popularity, humor, and multitalentedness. Teens rated likability, popularity, and attractiveness of vignette subjects in each domain. The sample was roughly evenly divided by gender (47% male and 53% female); students ranged in age from 11 to 18 years. The sample was ethnically diverse, and the socioeconomic status of the students' families ranged from lower middle to upper middle class. MANOVA analyses showed female vignette subjects to be rated significantly higher than male subjects by both genders and all grade groupings, at p < .0001 overall, and at values ranging from p < .05 to p < .0001 for six of nine subscales. Results are contrasted with those from highly similar studies 30 years ago, and used to reflect a possible shift in attitudes toward high achievement in women. 相似文献
983.
Behavioral versus Behavioral and Pharmacological Treatment in ADHD Children Attending a Summer Treatment Program 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Pelham WE Gnagy EM Greiner AR Hoza B Hinshaw SP Swanson JM Simpson S Shapiro C Bukstein O Baron-Myak C McBurnett K 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2000,28(6):507-525
As part of the behavioral treatment in the Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (MTA), children participated in an intensive summer treatment program (STP). This study examined the differences between 57 children in the combined treatment (Comb) group, who were medicated, and 60 children in the behavioral treatment (Beh) group, who were unmedicated throughout the STP. Comb children were significantly better than Beh on 5 measures: rule following, good sportsmanship, peer negative nominations, and STP teacher posttreatment ratings of inattention/overactivity. Groups did not differ on any of the other 30 measures, and responded similarly to the STP over time. Comparisons to normative data revealed that Comb children were more likely to fall within the normative range on 6 measures. The differences between these results and the main MTA results, in which Comb was always superior to Beh, are discussed in terms of the relative intensity of combined treatments. The implications for future studies of pharmacological and behavioral treatment for ADHD are discussed. 相似文献
984.
Cowan N Elliott EM Saults JS Nugent LD Bomb P Hismjatullina A 《Psychological science》2006,17(1):67-73
Researchers have suggested that developmental improvements in immediate recall stem from increases in the speed of mental processes. However, that inference has depended on evidence from correlation, regression, and structural equation modeling. We provide counter-examples in two experiments in which the speed of spoken recall was manipulated. In one experiment, second-grade children and adults recalled lists of digits more quickly than usual when the lists were presented at a rapid rate of two items per second. In a second experiment, children received lists at a rate of one item per second; half the children were trained (successfully) to speak their responses more quickly than usual, at a rate similar to adults' usual rate. Recall accuracy was completely unaffected by either of these response-speed manipulations. Thus, although response rate is a strong marker of an individual's maturational level, it does not appear to determine the accuracy of immediate recall. These results have important methodological and theoretical implications for human development. 相似文献
985.
This paper investigates the ability of infants to attend to continuous stimulus variables and how this capacity relates to the representation of number. We examined the change in area needed by 6-month-old infants to detect a difference in the size of a single element (Elmo face). Infants successfully discriminated a 1:4, 1:3 and 1:2 change in the area of the Elmo face but failed to discriminate a 2:3 change. In addition, the novelty preference was linearly related to the ratio difference between the novel and familiar area. Results suggest that Weber's Law holds for area discriminations in infancy and also reveal that at 6 months of age infants are equally sensitive to number, time and area. 相似文献
986.
Studzinski CM Christie LA Araujo JA Burnham WM Head E Cotman CW Milgram NW 《Neurobiology of learning and memory》2006,86(2):197-204
Visuospatial learning and memory impairments are an early marker for age-related cognitive decline and Alzheimer's disease. Similar to humans, aged dogs show visuospatial learning and memory deficits (). One hundred and nine beagle dogs ranging between 0.25 and 11.99 years were tested on a visuospatial delayed non-matching to position (DNMP) task to better characterize the progression of visuospatial deficits in the dog. Age predicted 48.2% of the variability in learning the DNMP, with dogs ranging from 1 to 11.99 years generally making more errors with increasing age. By contrast, puppies (<1 year) likely were showing developmental deficits, possibly due to an immature prefrontal cortex. Mild visuospatial deficits were detected by 6 years, which precedes the typical onset of amyloid-beta (Abeta) accumulation in the dog brain by two years, and can serve as an early marker for cognitive decline in the dog. These findings suggest that (1) age-related changes in visuospatial function in the dog models that seen in humans, further validating the dog as a model for human aging and dementia; and (2) other mechanisms, such as oxidative stress, soluble Abeta oligomers or cholinergic deficits, are likely contributing to the early impairment. 相似文献
987.
Implementing Family-Centered Interventions Within the Public Middle School: Linking Service Delivery to Change in Student Problem Behavior 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
The Adolescent Transitions Program is a family-centered intervention strategy designed to reduce problem behavior and prevent
drug use within a public school environment. A parent consultant within a Family Resource Center (FRC) provided universal,
selected, and indicated interventions that enhanced and supported positive parenting practices known to serve as protective
factors. Implementation of the 3-year FRC model involved 584 students and their families in 4 middle schools. Analyses focused
on the dynamics and effects of parent consultant activities and services. Differences in school “adoption” of the FRC services
were found to be a function of both passage of time and school environment. Despite significant variation in implementation
across schools, FRC services significantly reduced the growth in problem behavior over the course of the middle-school years.
The implications of these findings for dissemination of empirically supported, school-based, family-centered interventions
are discussed. 相似文献
988.
Capacity limits in short-term recall were investigated using proactive interference (PI) from previous lists in a speeded-recognition task. PI was taken to indicate that the target list length surpassed working memory capacity. Unlike previous studies, words were presented either concurrently or sequentially and a new method was introduced to increase the amount of PI. On average, participants retrieved about four items without PI. We suggest an activation-based account of capacity limits. 相似文献
989.
Roediger HL Marsh EJ 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2005,31(5):1155-1159
Multiple-choice tests are commonly used in educational settings but with unknown effects on students' knowledge. The authors examined the consequences of taking a multiple-choice test on a later general knowledge test in which students were warned not to guess. A large positive testing effect was obtained: Prior testing of facts aided final cued-recall performance. However, prior testing also had negative consequences. Prior reading of a greater number of multiple-choice lures decreased the positive testing effect and increased production of multiple-choice lures as incorrect answers on the final test. Multiple-choice testing may inadvertently lead to the creation of false knowledge. 相似文献
990.
This study examined the effect of provocateurs' emotion displays on first through fourth graders' social information processing (SIP). Rating and nomination sociometric techniques were used to identify rejected-aggressive, rejected-nonaggressive, average-nonaggressive, and popular-nonaggressive groups. Children viewed videotaped ambiguous provocation situations in which provocateurs' emotion displays were varied systematically. After each vignette, children's recall of story details, attribution of intent, and problem-solving responses were obtained. Half of the children were randomly assigned to a condition where they were asked directly about provocateurs' emotions, and the remaining children were not asked about provocateurs' feelings. Results revealed both developmental and social adjustment differences in SIP. In addition, provocateurs' emotion displays and asking versus not asking about those displays influenced multiple aspects of SIP. 相似文献