首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   44065篇
  免费   1069篇
  国内免费   14篇
  2020年   324篇
  2019年   336篇
  2018年   3844篇
  2017年   3152篇
  2016年   2688篇
  2015年   504篇
  2014年   507篇
  2013年   1936篇
  2012年   1307篇
  2011年   3125篇
  2010年   2887篇
  2009年   1953篇
  2008年   2366篇
  2007年   2782篇
  2006年   695篇
  2005年   776篇
  2004年   725篇
  2003年   654篇
  2002年   559篇
  2001年   865篇
  2000年   878篇
  1999年   632篇
  1998年   283篇
  1997年   256篇
  1996年   234篇
  1995年   226篇
  1992年   506篇
  1991年   445篇
  1990年   506篇
  1989年   421篇
  1988年   399篇
  1987年   381篇
  1986年   396篇
  1985年   464篇
  1984年   356篇
  1983年   302篇
  1982年   243篇
  1979年   340篇
  1978年   267篇
  1977年   219篇
  1976年   214篇
  1975年   312篇
  1974年   356篇
  1973年   362篇
  1972年   277篇
  1971年   248篇
  1969年   234篇
  1968年   290篇
  1967年   252篇
  1966年   273篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
941.
942.
This portion of a multiyear prospective study was designed to investigate the communicative style, speaking rate, and language complexity of 93 mothers of preschool children with a parental history of stuttering. At the initial session none of the children sampled was regarded as being a stutterer. One year later, 26 of the children were classified as stutterers. Statistical analyses revealed that prior to the onset of stuttering the mothers of these children did not differ from the mothers of the children who continued to be seen as nonstutterers with respect to either communicative style or speaking rate. The only significant difference between the two groups of mothers was the complexity of their language. The pre-onset mean lengths of utterance (MLUs) of the mothers of children who later came to be regarded as stutterers were significantly shorter than those of the mothers whose children continued to be viewed as being fluent. These findings suggest that the communicative behavior of mothers of normally fluent children do not contribute to the development of stuttering.  相似文献   
943.
Bullfrog tadpoles with cervical or midthoracic transection of the spinal cord were allowed to recover for 5 weeks, at which time axonal growth across the transection site was assessed by transport of horseradish peroxidase. Weekly behavioral tests included those for posture, spontaneous locomotion, cutaneously elicited swimming, and intersegmental coordination. Behavioral and electrophysiological assessments suggest that behavioral recovery depends, at least in part, on the growth of fibers across the transection site. Anatomical and behavioral recovery does not appear to differ with the level of spinal transection, but there was greater sparing of posture, spontaneous locomotion, and stimulus-induced locomotion in tadpoles with thoracic transection of the spinal cords.  相似文献   
944.
This paper provides outcome data about the efficacy of a behavioral group therapy program for obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Thirty-six patients completed a 7-week outpatient treatment program which provides: (1) education about OCD and its treatment, (2) instruction in a cognitive and behavioral approach to the self-treatment of OCD and (3) guided behavioral treatment. Significant improvement in obsession, compulsion, and depression scores were observed at completion of the group and at 3 month follow-up. Significant improvement in obsessions and compulsions was observed for both patients taking anti-obsessional medications and those who did not.  相似文献   
945.
946.
947.
Abstract: Do memories change as we acquire new information? Recent research on memory distortion using implicit tests along with research using confidence is reviewed and new studies are presented. Two new studies asked misinformed subjects to provide reasons for their answers. In each study 15% to 27% of subjects said they remembered seeing items they had only read about. In another study subjects were asked to identify the source of misleading items they had seen in slides or read in misleading questions. Subjects were more likely to say they had seen in slides something they read about in the questions than they were to confuse information from two nearly identical sets of slides. Recent work shows that, not only is it possible to distort memory for events, it is possible to implant an entire memory for something that never happened. The evidence is now clear that we can become mentally tricked into making large as well as small changes in the way we recall the past.  相似文献   
948.
949.
This research examines the applicability of Fishbein and Ajzen's (1975) theory of reasoned action (TRA) to intentions to use cigarettes and alcohol among 5th- and 6th-grade students. It also addresses the question of unidimensionality of outcome beliefs and referent norms. Results suggest that the TRA model describes children's decision making well, although the ability of income and race to add to prediction, net of the effects of attitude and perceived norm, are contrary to the theory's prediction. In addition, we find support for multidimensional components of both attitude and norm for both behaviors.  相似文献   
950.
Nevin's scholarly and timely discussion attempts to maintain the precarious union of the experimentally derived theory of behavioral momentum with applications of the behavioral momentum construct to human subjects in applied settings. Nevin's discussion adds much-needed clarification to a process that at times has proven to be awkward for applied researchers. In this commentary, we will address three general questions: (a) Can the applications of behavioral momentum be derived from the theory as conceptualized by Nevin? (b) Could those applications have been developed had the theory not been formulated? (c) Does behavioral momentum theory add significantly to the compliance literature?  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号