首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   12053篇
  免费   535篇
  国内免费   5篇
  2021年   123篇
  2020年   185篇
  2019年   198篇
  2018年   361篇
  2017年   313篇
  2016年   331篇
  2015年   213篇
  2014年   236篇
  2013年   936篇
  2012年   371篇
  2011年   400篇
  2010年   255篇
  2009年   236篇
  2008年   341篇
  2007年   312篇
  2006年   286篇
  2005年   241篇
  2004年   212篇
  2003年   190篇
  2002年   196篇
  2001年   419篇
  2000年   397篇
  1999年   270篇
  1998年   112篇
  1995年   103篇
  1993年   100篇
  1992年   214篇
  1991年   218篇
  1990年   207篇
  1989年   202篇
  1988年   210篇
  1987年   189篇
  1986年   174篇
  1985年   177篇
  1984年   161篇
  1983年   118篇
  1982年   127篇
  1979年   154篇
  1978年   96篇
  1977年   100篇
  1975年   138篇
  1974年   161篇
  1973年   185篇
  1972年   156篇
  1971年   144篇
  1970年   127篇
  1969年   134篇
  1968年   176篇
  1967年   134篇
  1966年   141篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
181.
182.
REMINISCENCES,YOU SAY?   总被引:2,自引:2,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
  相似文献   
183.
The proliferation of computer-generated psychological reports prompts an examination of the presuppositions of such reports and what they offer in the way of understanding individual clients. This topic is approached primarily through a review of some basics of psychological testing and psychological assessment. Major differences are noted between these two approaches to evaluating clients: They are different processes, and they yield different products. Assessment is a complex set of activities that seeks solutions to specific problems, whereas testing fits a straightforward actuarial paradigm. Testing is readily adapted to an automated data processing system, whereas the complex of assessment procedures cannot be so managed. Serious questions about the validity of computer reports are raised, however, and ethical concerns are expressed over the use of these reports by persons without adequate psychological training to understand them correctly. Limited uses for computer reports are suggested along with the need for further studies of validity and ethics.  相似文献   
184.
185.
186.
187.
188.
The representation of nonuniform motion: induced movement   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
N J Wade  M T Swanston 《Perception》1987,16(5):555-571
Induced motion occurs when there is a misallocation of nonuniform motion. Theories of induced motion are reviewed with respect to the model for uniform motion recently proposed by Swanston, Wade, and Day. Theories based on single processes operating at one of the retinocentric, orbitocentric, egocentric, or geocentric levels are not able to account for all aspects of the phenomenon. It is therefore suggested that induced motion is a consequence of combining two different types of motion signals: one provides information by registering the motion with respect to the retina, orbit, and egocentre; the other provides information only on the relational motions between the pattern elements. Simple rules are given for defining a frame of reference for the relational motion process, which can result in a reallocation of the motion signals. It is proposed that the two signals in combination are weighted differentially, with the greater influence coming from the relational signals. Procedures for determining the weighting factors are described, and predictions from the model are examined.  相似文献   
189.
Marianne N. Bloch 《Sex roles》1987,16(5-6):279-301
Randomly scheduled spot observations of 83 zero to six-year-old American middle-class children were used to investigate factors affecting the development of sex differences in young children's activities in and near their home. Sex and age differences in children's typical play and nonplay activities and in the people with whom they did activities were examined. The results showed that there were fewer sex differences in children's activities, activity partners, and general social settings than expected. Girls spent more time engaged in housework and school-related activities than boys, while boys spent more time in social gross motor play with other children and in self-care activities than girls. Children spent a surprisingly large amount of their time with members of their immediate family — particularly their mother and one other sibling. Boys and girls did not differ, however, in the time they were in settings or doing activities with their father, with their mother, or with same-or mixed-sex child groups.This research was supported by an award to the author from the Wisconsin Alumnae Research Foundation at the University of Wisconsin — Madison. The author would also like to acknowledge the research and editorial assistance of Daniel J. Walsh. In addition, Aletha Huston, Beatrice B. Whiting, and Gary G. Price provided valuable advice on this and earlier versions of this paper.  相似文献   
190.
This study examined the effects of two social cues on women's and men's self-confidence: the sex and performance of another in an achievement setting. Before trying to solve 60 anagrams, women expected to perform more poorly than men. In addition, both sexes expected to perform more poorly when paired with a male confederate than when paired with a female confederate. The effect of the partner's sex showed up in anagram performance: people with a female partner solved more anagrams than people with a male partner. The effect of subject's sex on performance was unexpected: women solved more anagrams than men. After the task, sex differences in self-confidence disappeared; neither sex of subject nor sex of partner influenced self-confidence. The partner's performance, however, had a strong effect on everyone: people were less self-confident if they had worked with a high-scoring partner. The pattern of results suggests that sex-of-subject differences in self-confidence, while important, are less powerful than the effects wielded by the sex of others in achievement settings.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号