首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   619篇
  免费   18篇
  637篇
  2023年   4篇
  2020年   15篇
  2019年   9篇
  2018年   16篇
  2017年   18篇
  2016年   25篇
  2015年   20篇
  2014年   18篇
  2013年   50篇
  2012年   22篇
  2011年   26篇
  2010年   21篇
  2009年   17篇
  2008年   28篇
  2007年   23篇
  2006年   19篇
  2005年   14篇
  2004年   17篇
  2003年   15篇
  2002年   15篇
  2001年   8篇
  2000年   6篇
  1999年   5篇
  1998年   10篇
  1997年   9篇
  1995年   13篇
  1994年   15篇
  1993年   8篇
  1992年   9篇
  1991年   7篇
  1990年   7篇
  1989年   8篇
  1988年   9篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   8篇
  1985年   11篇
  1984年   9篇
  1983年   8篇
  1982年   8篇
  1981年   8篇
  1980年   7篇
  1979年   8篇
  1978年   6篇
  1976年   4篇
  1975年   6篇
  1974年   4篇
  1972年   6篇
  1971年   4篇
  1970年   4篇
  1967年   5篇
排序方式: 共有637条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
92.
93.
Criteria used to evaluate socially competent behavier of women are described and critiqued. Many definitions of social competence do not employ a process view of social behavier in which individual goals and values as well as specific verbal and nonverbal behaviers are considered. This may result in the imposition of artificial goals on consumers of assertion and social skills training programs and the neglect of individual goals. The relationship between the definition of competence used and the assessment methods relied on is discussed. The advantages of focusing on specific goals are noted and a checklist that consumers can use to review the content of training programs is provided.  相似文献   
94.
95.
96.
Responses to items such as brane are slower and/or more error prone than responses to items such as slint in lexical decision (is this string spelt like a real word?). The received view is that this “pseudohomophone” effect is attributable to phonological receding. Taft (1982) has challenged this view, offering instead a grapheme-grapheme account which assumes that graphemes that map onto a common phoneme develop the ability to activate each other without reference to phonological mediation.

Taft's grapheme-grapheme account is tested in two experiments. Experiment 1 shows that the presentation of a pseudohomophone facilitates the response to a subsequently presented word (e.g., groce-gross). Experiment 2 shows that nonword letter strings that are translatable into words by the application of putative grapheme-grapheme rules (e.g., gloce-gloss) produce no facilitation. These results are consistent with the notion of a phonological influence but inconsistent with the grapheme-grapheme account. Loci for this pseudohomophone priming effect are discussed.  相似文献   
97.
    
  相似文献   
98.
Bennett  David S.  Borczon  Eileen  Lewis  Michael 《Sex roles》2019,81(7-8):521-528
Sex Roles - Gender nonconforming behavior (GNB) is a risk factor for poorer psychological adjustment, but little is known about whether preschool-age children displaying GNB are at risk for...  相似文献   
99.
During source remembering, selectively emphasizing one source in the retrieval query “Is this item from Source A?” can yield different accuracy than emphasizing the alternate source in “Is this item from Source B?” even if those are the only two possible origins. One account of this cue-framing effect holds that it reflects different active monitoring strategies encouraged by the two cue frames. An item memory misattribution (IMM) model instead assumes that this effect reflects the uncontrolled use of item recognition during confirmatory source judgments, and an IMM model simulation predicted a quantitative relationship between recognition levels and the cue-framing effect. Experiments 1 and 3 confirmed these predictions by using study repetitions to manipulate recognition levels, and Experiments 2 and 3 also demonstrated the effect with new source tasks not previously considered. The data suggest that, in addition to qualitative monitoring strategies, subjects also use the availability of item memory in a heuristic fashion during confirmatory source attributions.  相似文献   
100.
We review evidence of inequality associated with gender and social categories like ethnicity with respect to six types of power: control of resources, force, legitimacy, obligations, knowledge, and sexuality. Presuming that stereotypes are one means of legitimizing power differentials between groups, we review research on stereotype contents for both ethnicity and gender with respect to the six forms of power. The review reveals a number of rhetorical means by which stereotypes legitimize ethnic dominants and men having particular forms of power, such as disguising the exercise of power by describing it in individualistic rather than relational terms, and also how stereotypes de‐legitimize women and ethnic subordinates from obtaining particular forms of power, such as by stating that what they desire and their virtues preclude exercising power. A new research agenda regarding stereotypes and how they legitimize group power differences is outlined.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号