首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   26篇
  免费   2篇
  2021年   2篇
  2020年   1篇
  2018年   1篇
  2017年   1篇
  2016年   1篇
  2015年   1篇
  2013年   3篇
  2009年   1篇
  1996年   1篇
  1995年   1篇
  1994年   1篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   1篇
  1990年   1篇
  1986年   1篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   1篇
  1978年   1篇
  1974年   1篇
  1973年   1篇
  1970年   2篇
  1967年   3篇
排序方式: 共有28条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
12.
Innovations in software for the analysis of eye movements have not kept pace with the development of hardware for collecting samples of eye position (Scinto & Barnette, 1986). Eye fixation and duration have been the primary measures for gleaning knowledge of subjects’ performance while the subjects are engaged in cognitive visual tasks. The program Cluster was developed as a means of investigating the dynamics of target examination characteristics that do not lend themselves to traditional methods of eye-movement analysis. This tool has proved to be a valuable means of assessing visual activity at a micro level, in comparison with the gross measures of distribution of visual attention in various areas of the visual field. In this report, we describe the history surrounding the development of Cluster as an analytical tool, the source of input required for its execution, the mechanics of the execution as an interactive process, the replicability of raters’ judgments, the program’s products of visual displays and data file output, and the potential application of such a tool for analyzing visual activity.  相似文献   
13.
A recent debate over trauma theory in Holocaust studies has implications for writing by the “generation after.” This article looks at Anne Karpf’s The War After (1996) and the 1998 movie Left Luggage (based on a novel by Carl Friedman), as well as David Grossman’s See Under: Love (1985), as examples of how traumatic knowledge is accessed through literary narratives and the imagination, rather than the historical events. In particular, the trope of “autism” serves to symbolize the difficulty of communicating repressed traumatic memory, while images of containment symbolize the unspeakable contents of the psychic envelope.  相似文献   
14.
15.
16.
Persons with developmental disabilities often do not follow instructions or complete tasks in educational settings, thus further disadvantaging themselves as they try to lead more normalized lives. The purpose of this paper was to address the problems of three persons referred for not following instructions and for engaging in associated behavior problems (e.g., crying, hitting, throwing materials) when given instructions. A functional assessment was conducted in the natural school setting without any prescribed conditions as in analogue functional assessments. Results of the assessments showed that problem behaviors for two subjects occurred much more in demand conditions requiring active responses than in demand conditions requiring passive responses. The results for a third subject showed problem behaviors were associated with toileting demands, less so with other demand conditions, but not with conditions in which there were an absence of demands. Individualized multi-element interventions were based on the negative reinforcement hypothesis and were intended to make tasks less aversive and to make instructions easier to understand. Results of the interventions showed increases in task engagement and decreases in problem behaviors during the active tasks and toileting tasks for which interventions were programmed, as well as for the passive tasks and other demand conditions for which no interventions were programmed. Results were discussed in terms of the value of functional assessments in directing therapists to base interventions on hypotheses of the function of the presenting problem.  相似文献   
17.
A comparison was made between two procedures for teaching persons with severe handicaps: (a) the task demonstration model, which is based upon a fading procedure and general case programming, and (b) the standard prompting hierarchy, a least-to-most intrusive prompting procedure commonly used to teach these individuals. Five phases were used in comparing the procedures: pretesting, training, two generalization tests, and a 6-month maintenance test. Eight students learned two discrimination tasks by each procedure, with each task involving two- or three-digit numbers. Results showed that under the task demonstration model all 8 subjects had more unprompted correct responses (about 1.5 times as many) in training, all 8 subjects had fewer errors (about 0.6 times as many) in training, all 8 subjects had more correct responding in the generalization test with untrained stimuli in the training room, 6 of 8 subjects had more correct responding with untrained stimuli in another room, a 7th had equivalent amounts, and 7 of 8 subjects had more correct responding on a 6-month maintenance test. Thus, the task demonstration model proved superior to the standard prompting hierarchy in 29 of 32 tests of correct responding. Results are discussed in terms of implications for stimulus control training strategies.  相似文献   
18.
19.
Although pride and joy are both positive emotions, we expected their consideration to affect persuasion differently because of the different perspectives (near vs. distant) and level of abstractness they involve, with pride being more abstract than joy. Therefore, we predicted that when the attitude object is construed at a high level rather than a low level, the consideration of pride is likely to promote more persuasion than the consideration of joy. In three studies, we found that the consideration of pride, when featured in the persuasion message (Studies 1a and 1b) or incidentally (Study 2), increased persuasion more than did the consideration of joy, when the persuasion object was temporally distant compared with temporally near (Studies 1a and 1b) or construed as a high‐level category compared with a more concrete individual (Study 2). These findings advance our understanding of the ways in which specific emotions may affect persuasion, beyond valence. Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
20.
A new apparatus, suitable for obtaining reliable reports of entoptic images, was developed. Basically, this apparatus moved a small light across the S’s eyelid (transcleral illumination). While the light was moving, Ss reported the appearance of the three entoptic phenomena of interest-retinal blood vessels, foveal images, and background illumination. After initial training with this apparatus, 12 Ss were tested for the differential disappearance (fading) of the entoptic images after the light source stopped moving but continued its transcleral illumination of the retina. Results indicated that although the background image did fade to a dark field, this background fade time ( \(\bar X\) = 1.3 sec) is faster than that reported for externally stabilized images, which may take 2 to 4 sec to fade, and was significantly slower than the very rapid fade times for blood vessels and foveal images. Furthermore, there was no reappearance of these entoptic images.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号