全文获取类型
收费全文 | 8654篇 |
免费 | 166篇 |
专业分类
8820篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 93篇 |
2019年 | 124篇 |
2018年 | 151篇 |
2017年 | 163篇 |
2016年 | 155篇 |
2015年 | 133篇 |
2014年 | 169篇 |
2013年 | 745篇 |
2012年 | 279篇 |
2011年 | 321篇 |
2010年 | 196篇 |
2009年 | 199篇 |
2008年 | 223篇 |
2007年 | 222篇 |
2006年 | 220篇 |
2005年 | 185篇 |
2004年 | 187篇 |
2003年 | 168篇 |
2002年 | 191篇 |
2001年 | 229篇 |
2000年 | 225篇 |
1999年 | 194篇 |
1998年 | 98篇 |
1997年 | 79篇 |
1993年 | 84篇 |
1992年 | 148篇 |
1991年 | 136篇 |
1990年 | 133篇 |
1989年 | 118篇 |
1988年 | 117篇 |
1987年 | 114篇 |
1986年 | 119篇 |
1985年 | 138篇 |
1984年 | 129篇 |
1983年 | 92篇 |
1981年 | 103篇 |
1980年 | 96篇 |
1979年 | 136篇 |
1978年 | 107篇 |
1977年 | 95篇 |
1976年 | 89篇 |
1975年 | 123篇 |
1974年 | 140篇 |
1973年 | 123篇 |
1972年 | 97篇 |
1971年 | 91篇 |
1970年 | 89篇 |
1969年 | 98篇 |
1968年 | 89篇 |
1967年 | 104篇 |
排序方式: 共有8820条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
981.
982.
983.
Mary T. Hawkins Kristi-Ann Villagonzalo Ann V. Sanson John W. Toumbourou Primrose Letcher Craig A. Olsson 《Journal of Adult Development》2012,19(2):88-99
Little is known about positive development across the transition from adolescence to young adulthood. The purpose of this paper was to use longitudinal data from a large community sample, the Australian Temperament Project, to investigate prospective associations between positive development at age 19?C20?years and changes in social, health, and behavioral outcomes at 23?C24?years. Data from 890 young adults (61.7% women) were analyzed using path analysis. Emotional health, physical well-being, friendship quality, reduced antisocial behavior, and positive development in young adulthood were predicted by higher levels of positive development in late adolescence, after controlling for baseline levels of respective outcomes, gender, and family socioeconomic status. Parent relationship quality and alcohol use were not significantly predicted. Based on these findings, interventions promoting positive development during adolescence should be investigated as a potential means of yielding positive social, health, and behavioral consequences well into young adulthood. 相似文献
984.
985.
A cross-cultural investigation into the relationships between personality traits and work values 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Over 500 working individuals from 2 European countries (Great Britain and Greece) participated in a 2-study investigation into the relationships between the Big Five personality factors and individuals' work values. In Study 1 (N = 314), British employees completed the 60-item NEO-Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; P. T. Costa & R. R. McCrae, 1989) and the Work Values Questionnaire (WVQ; Mantech, 1983), which comprises 37 items measuring intrinsic (e.g., autonomy) and extrinsic (e.g., pay) work values. In Study 2 (N = 216), Greek employees completed a Greek language measure of the Big Five and a translated version of the WVQ. The authors observed a similar factor structure for the WVQ items in both studies. Personality traits, age, and gender explained between 5% and 13% of the variance in the WVQ subscales. As a result of the 2 studies, the authors concluded that there are robust associations between certain personality traits and work values, although they do not clearly follow the intrinsic vs. extrinsic distinction. 相似文献
986.
K. C. Klauer, J. Musch, and B. Naumer (2000; see record 2000-02818-008) presented a general multinomial model of belief bias effects in syllogistic reasoning. They claimed to map a particular mental model account of belief bias (J. V. Oakhill, P. N. Johnson-Laird, & A. Garnham, 1989; see record 1989-38845-001)) onto this model and to show empirically that it is incorrect. The authors argue that this mental model account does not map onto the multinomial model and that it can account for the data presented by Klauer et al. (Experiments 1-4). The authors further argue that additional data Klauer et al. presented in support of a new model of their own (Experiments 5-8) are explained by this mental model account. The mental model account is, therefore, refuted neither by Klauer et al.'s theoretical analysis nor by any of the results they presented. Furthermore, the account can accommodate more recent findings on belief bias in a more satisfactory way than can alternative models that have been proposed. 相似文献
987.
In order to determine the reflectance of a surface, it is necessary to discount luminance changes produced by illumination variation, a process that requires the visual system to respond differently to luminance changes that are due to illumination and reflectance. It is known that various cues can be used in this process. By measuring the strength of lightness illusions, we find evidence that straightness is, used as a cue: When a boundary is straight rather than curved, it has a greater tendency to be discounted, as if it were an illumination edge. The strongest illusions occur when a boundary has high contrast and has multiple X-junctions that preserve a consistent contrast ratio. 相似文献
988.
Sangrigoli S Pallier C Argenti AM Ventureyra VA de Schonen S 《Psychological science》2005,16(6):440-444
Early experience with faces of a given racial type facilitates visual recognition for this type of face relative to others. To assess whether this so-called other-race effect can be reversed by subsequent experience with new types of faces, we tested adults of Korean origin who were adopted by European Caucasian families when they were between the ages of 3 to 9. The adoptees performed a face recognition task with photographs of Caucasian and Asian faces. They performed exactly like a control group of French participants, identifying the Caucasian faces better than the Asiatic ones. In contrast, a control group of Koreans showed the reverse pattern. This result indicates that the face recognition system remains plastic enough during childhood to reverse the other-race effect. 相似文献
989.
Investigations of childhood amnesia have almost exclusively focused on the earliest memories of adults. Here we investigate the earliest memories of children of 6--19 years old. Parents confirmed the memory events and dated the memories. There were surprisingly few developmental differences between the earliest memories of children. Although 6--9-year-olds recalled earlier events than did older children, there were no differences between older age groups. Memories from all age groups were similar in structure, social orientation, and the nature of the recalled event. However, memories of older children were more likely to involve negative affect. There were also few gender differences, although girls were more likely to recall traumatic or transitional events while boys were more likely to recall play events. Overall, results deepen the paradox of early memory: 6--9-year-olds have verbally accessible memories from very early childhood that then seem to disappear as they get older. 相似文献
990.