全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2318篇 |
免费 | 79篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
2398篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 20篇 |
2020年 | 21篇 |
2019年 | 31篇 |
2018年 | 38篇 |
2017年 | 45篇 |
2016年 | 36篇 |
2015年 | 29篇 |
2014年 | 58篇 |
2013年 | 206篇 |
2012年 | 70篇 |
2011年 | 92篇 |
2010年 | 53篇 |
2009年 | 68篇 |
2008年 | 69篇 |
2007年 | 72篇 |
2006年 | 76篇 |
2005年 | 71篇 |
2004年 | 74篇 |
2003年 | 57篇 |
2002年 | 70篇 |
2001年 | 27篇 |
2000年 | 32篇 |
1999年 | 39篇 |
1998年 | 42篇 |
1997年 | 34篇 |
1996年 | 29篇 |
1995年 | 31篇 |
1994年 | 28篇 |
1993年 | 30篇 |
1992年 | 37篇 |
1991年 | 24篇 |
1990年 | 26篇 |
1989年 | 24篇 |
1988年 | 28篇 |
1987年 | 24篇 |
1986年 | 23篇 |
1985年 | 37篇 |
1984年 | 39篇 |
1983年 | 21篇 |
1982年 | 29篇 |
1981年 | 45篇 |
1980年 | 40篇 |
1979年 | 27篇 |
1978年 | 51篇 |
1977年 | 43篇 |
1976年 | 27篇 |
1975年 | 47篇 |
1974年 | 37篇 |
1973年 | 33篇 |
1972年 | 18篇 |
排序方式: 共有2398条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
Watkins (2004) found that the mode of processing adopted during expressive writing following a failure influenced emotional recovery from the failure as a function of level of trait rumination. At higher levels of trait rumination, negative mood 12 hours after the failure was greater, but only in an abstract, evaluative writing condition and not in a concrete, process-focused condition. The current study examined whether this interaction of trait rumination with processing mode would generalize to emotional vulnerability to a subsequent negative stressor. Participants repeatedly focused on both positive and negative scenarios in either a concrete, process-focused or an abstract, evaluative mode, before a failure experience. As predicted, after the failure experience, higher levels of trait rumination were associated with lower levels of positive affect, but only for participants in the abstract, evaluative condition and not for participants in the concrete, process-focused condition. This finding is consistent with processing mode influencing the relationship between trait rumination and emotional vulnerability. 相似文献
92.
The prepared mind: neural activity prior to problem presentation predicts subsequent solution by sudden insight 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Kounios J Frymiare JL Bowden EM Fleck JI Subramaniam K Parrish TB Jung-Beeman M 《Psychological science》2006,17(10):882-890
Insight occurs when problem solutions arise suddenly and seem obviously correct, and is associated with an "Aha!" experience. Prior theorizing concerning preparation that facilitates insight focused on solvers' problem-specific knowledge. We hypothesized that a distinct type of mental preparation, manifested in a distinct brain state, would facilitate insight problem solving independently of problem-specific knowledge. Consistent with this hypothesis, neural activity during a preparatory interval before subjects saw verbal problems predicted which problems they would subsequently solve with, versus without, self-reported insight. Specifically, electroencephalographic topography and frequency (Experiment 1) and functional magnetic resonance imaging signal (Experiment 2) both suggest that mental preparation leading to insight involves heightened activity in medial frontal areas associated with cognitive control and in temporal areas associated with semantic processing. The results for electroencephalographic topography suggest that noninsight preparation, in contrast, involves increased occipital activity consistent with an increase in externally directed visual attention. Thus, general preparatory mechanisms modulate problem-solving strategy. 相似文献
93.
Monahan J Steadman HJ Appelbaum PS Grisso T Mulvey EP Roth LH Robbins PC Banks S Silver E 《Behavioral sciences & the law》2006,24(6):721-730
The Classification of Violence Risk (COVR) is an interactive software program designed to estimate the risk that a person hospitalized for mental disorder will be violent to others. The software leads the evaluator through a chart review and a brief interview with the patient. At the end of this interview, the software generates a report that contains a statistically valid estimate of the patient's violence risk-ranging from a 1% to a 76% likelihood of violence-including the confidence interval for that estimate, and a list of the risk factors that the program took into account to produce the estimate. In this article, the development of the COVR software is described and several issues that arise in its administration are discussed. 相似文献
94.
We explored college students' discrimination of complex visual stimuli that involvedmultiple-item displays. The items in each of the displays could be all the same, all different, or diverse mixtures of some same and some different items. The participants had to learn which of two arbitrary responses was correct for each of the displays without being told about the sameness or differentness of the stimuli. We observed a general improvement in discrimination performance--a rise in choice accuracy and a fall in reaction time-as the number of icons in the display was increased, even when the participants had been trained from the outset with displays containing different numbers of items and when smaller numbers of items were not randomly distributed but grouped in the center of the display. The participants' discrimination behavior also depended on the mixture of same and different items in the displays. Striking individual differences in the participants' discrimination behavior disclosed that people sometimes respond as do pigeons and baboons trained with a similar task. This and previous related research suggest that variability discrimination may lie at the root of same-different categorization behavior. 相似文献
95.
Age differences in fluid intelligence: contributions of general slowing and frontal decline 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
The current study examined the contributions of general slowing and frontal decline to age differences in fluid intelligence. Participants aged 20-89 years completed Block Design, Matrix Reasoning, simple reaction time, choice reaction time, Wisconsin Card Sorting, and Tower of London tasks. Age-related declines in fluid intelligence, speed of processing, and frontal function were observed. Hierarchical regression analyses showed that the processing speed and frontal function measures accounted for significant variance in fluid intelligence performance, but there was also a residual effect of age after controlling for each variable individually as well as both variables. An additional analysis showed that the variance in fluid intelligence that was attributable to processing speed was not fully shared with the variance attributable to frontal function. These findings suggest that the age-related decline in fluid intelligence is due to general slowing and frontal decline, as well as other unidentified factors. 相似文献
96.
Three studies found that prospective temporal self-appraisals can be part of defensive pessimists' strategy; they felt closer to equally distant negative than positive futures. In Study 1, defensive pessimists felt closer to future failures and reported more negative affect than those considering success. In Study 2, when manipulated negative futures were close, defensive pessimists felt bad and performed well; results suggested that viewing negative futures as close may be part of their natural strategy. Study 3 found that prospective self-appraisals influenced performances through felt preparation. Optimists did not use prospective self-appraisals (Study 1) and their performances were unaffected by manipulated temporal distance (Studies 2 and 3). Discussion centers on prospective self-appraisals and multiple strategies of defensive pessimists. 相似文献
97.
Edward H. Fischer Ellen A. Dornelas Carolyn S. Malchodi Cheryl A. Oncken 《Journal of applied social psychology》2006,36(12):2825-2833
Two independent samples of pregnant smokers were interviewed about their cigarette smoking and were tested with biochemical markers at the beginning and end of their pregnancies. Participants were mainly unmarried, racial minorities (chiefly Hispanic), and residents of the inner city. Phi coefficients between self‐reported and chemical indicators ranged from .42 to .74. Contrary to expectations, the self‐report/biochemical‐measure correlations were higher at the end than at the beginning of the pregnancy term. The higher end‐of‐term correspondence between self‐report and chemical indicators likely reflected better reliability of the measures, when taken at term. The results are in accord with earlier research with other populations, indicating adequate validity for self‐reported smoking. 相似文献
98.
Interaction behaviors and language measures of low income mothers and their 12-month-old infants were compared for floor-play situations in which the mother was aware and unaware of being videotaped. When the mothers were aware of being videotaped, they were more proximal to their infants, offered and demonstrated toys more frequently, engaged in more frequent interaction games, vocalized more frequently, emitted a greater number of words as well as declarative and imperative utterances, and their infants engaged in more constructive play. Combining the analysis of variance and correlational analyses results suggested that the verbal behaviors of mothers were inflated and their non-verbal behaviors were distorted when they were aware of being videotaped. The implications of these data for the use of videotaping as an assessment and intervention tool are discussed. 相似文献
99.
Edward M. Burns 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1981,30(5):467-472
The Shepard illusion, in which the presentation of a cyclically repetitive sequence of complex tones composed of partials separated by octave intervals (Shepard, 1964) gives the illusion of an endlessly increasing sequence of pitch steps, is often cited as evidence for octave equivalence. In this paper, evidence is presented which demonstrates that this illusion can be produced using (inharmonic) complex tones whose partials are separated by equal ratios other than octaves. Therefore, the illusion is not evidence for octave equivalence. 相似文献
100.
Patrick Cavanagh Edward M. Brussell Stephen R. Stober 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1981,29(5):423-428
Following adaptation to a rectangular grating, subjects observed a shift in the perceived duty cycle of a test grating (Burton, Nagshineh, & Ruddock, 1977; DeValois, 1977). This suggests that size information about black and white pattern components may be analyzed independently. We have tested this hypothesis by combining adaptation to a 2-cpd rectangular wave grating (narrow white bars and wide black bars or vice versa) with adaptation to a 4-cpd sine wave (narrow white and black bars. The data do not support the independent analysis of white and black components, but they may be accounted for by mechanisms sensitive to the relative phase relations among the spatial frequency components of the input. 相似文献