首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2472篇
  免费   83篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2556篇
  2021年   20篇
  2020年   22篇
  2019年   33篇
  2018年   43篇
  2017年   49篇
  2016年   42篇
  2015年   29篇
  2014年   61篇
  2013年   230篇
  2012年   73篇
  2011年   96篇
  2010年   55篇
  2009年   69篇
  2008年   72篇
  2007年   79篇
  2006年   78篇
  2005年   74篇
  2004年   76篇
  2003年   58篇
  2002年   73篇
  2001年   31篇
  2000年   35篇
  1999年   39篇
  1998年   42篇
  1997年   35篇
  1996年   30篇
  1995年   32篇
  1994年   32篇
  1993年   31篇
  1992年   38篇
  1991年   28篇
  1990年   28篇
  1989年   28篇
  1988年   35篇
  1987年   29篇
  1986年   27篇
  1985年   40篇
  1984年   42篇
  1983年   23篇
  1982年   31篇
  1981年   49篇
  1980年   44篇
  1979年   29篇
  1978年   54篇
  1977年   45篇
  1976年   28篇
  1975年   49篇
  1974年   39篇
  1973年   33篇
  1972年   20篇
排序方式: 共有2556条查询结果,搜索用时 3 毫秒
81.
82.
The relative satiation effect, an inverse relationship between the frequency of prior social reinforcement (the word “good”) and the later effectiveness of the social reinforcer in controlling behavior, was studied. In Experiment 1, a discrimination task in which social reinforcement was given for correct responses was administered to first- to fourth-grade children (6 to 10 years of age), who had during a preexposure phase performed a preliminary task or observed another child performing. During the preexposure phase, the experimenter delivered frequent or infrequent social reinforcement that was either contingent or noncontingent. Only performers and observers who had experienced frequent noncontingent reinforcement showed the satiation effect during the discrimination task phase. The results were interpreted as inconsistent with J. L. Gewirtz' (Developmental Psychology, 1969, 1, 2–13) social drive formulation but supportive of an informational analysis in which the children are seen as responding appropriately to unambiguous evidence concerning the reliability of contingency information. In Experiment 2, seating arrangements were varied so that information concerning the direction of reinforcement was made ambiguous. Performers were less responsive during the discrimination phase after experiencing frequent noncontingent reinforcement when seated alone or opposite an observer than when seated next to an observer. The results are interpreted as indicating trust of the reliability of the contingency under ambiguous conditions.  相似文献   
83.
Infants of 5–6 months of age were tested for recognition of briefly presented photographs of faces. The interaction typically obtained with adults, a beneficial effect on retention due to the temporal spacing of study, was obtained with these infants. The results suggest that the distribution effect reflects a fundamental and automatic process of human memory.  相似文献   
84.
85.
86.
87.
88.
The effects of instructional set (become aroused or suppress arousal), stimulus modality (videotape, audiotape, slides, free fantasy) and stimulus content (paraphilic or non-paraphilic) on sexual arousal, as measured by penile plethysmograph, of six different groups of paraphilics, pedophiles, rapists, exhibitionists, homosexuals, sado-masochists and fetishists, were determined. The results showed significant main effects of instructional set and of stimulus modality (videotape was clearly more arousing) as well as several complex interactions of diagnostic group with the other independent variables.  相似文献   
89.
The Shepard illusion, in which the presentation of a cyclically repetitive sequence of complex tones composed of partials separated by octave intervals (Shepard, 1964) gives the illusion of an endlessly increasing sequence of pitch steps, is often cited as evidence for octave equivalence. In this paper, evidence is presented which demonstrates that this illusion can be produced using (inharmonic) complex tones whose partials are separated by equal ratios other than octaves. Therefore, the illusion is not evidence for octave equivalence.  相似文献   
90.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号