首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1182篇
  免费   49篇
  1231篇
  2023年   9篇
  2021年   15篇
  2020年   25篇
  2019年   15篇
  2018年   28篇
  2017年   36篇
  2016年   29篇
  2015年   22篇
  2014年   19篇
  2013年   124篇
  2012年   55篇
  2011年   57篇
  2010年   25篇
  2009年   43篇
  2008年   58篇
  2007年   34篇
  2006年   37篇
  2005年   34篇
  2004年   24篇
  2003年   33篇
  2002年   26篇
  2001年   20篇
  2000年   19篇
  1999年   13篇
  1998年   17篇
  1997年   15篇
  1996年   10篇
  1995年   15篇
  1994年   16篇
  1993年   13篇
  1992年   14篇
  1991年   16篇
  1990年   21篇
  1989年   19篇
  1985年   13篇
  1984年   11篇
  1983年   12篇
  1982年   17篇
  1981年   12篇
  1980年   11篇
  1979年   15篇
  1978年   13篇
  1976年   11篇
  1975年   12篇
  1972年   10篇
  1971年   12篇
  1970年   9篇
  1969年   9篇
  1968年   11篇
  1967年   11篇
排序方式: 共有1231条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
121.
122.
123.
Edgar Zilsel 《Erkenntnis》1932,3(1):143-161
Ohne Zusammenfassung  相似文献   
124.
125.
An important focus of recent aggression research has been to identify personality variables that influence the expression of aggression. One such variable may be anxiety sensitivity (AS). Individuals high in AS fear unpleasant anxiety-related physiological sensations and perhaps physiological arousal in general. Accordingly, people high in AS are motivated to avoid situations that produce these sensations. With respect to aggressive encounters, an intense attack by an opponent involves significant physiological arousal. High anxiety-sensitive individuals may therefore attempt to decrease the intensity of the interaction by responding in a non-aggressive or conciliatory manner. To test this possibility, 112 community volunteers completed a measure of AS and a well-validated laboratory task designed to assess aggressive behavior. As predicted, an inverse relation between AS and extreme aggressive responding was found. Specifically, as AS increases, retaliatory aggression is less likely after intense attack by an opponent.  相似文献   
126.
Implicit memory processes were investigated via picture naming in healthy young and older adults and in persons with mild Alzheimer's disease (AD). Repetition priming in picture-naming was intact in all groups over the course of a short retention interval (seconds), and only the AD group revealed a deficit over a longer interval (72 hours). In addition, the AD group showed impaired procedural memory, with no benefit of practice on picture-naming. Impaired long-term priming was related to severity of AD. Both theoretical and methodological implications are discussed.  相似文献   
127.
128.
We are often required to interpret discordant emotional signals. Whereas equivalent cognitive paradigms cause noticeable conflict via their behavioral and psychophysiological effects, the same may not necessarily be true for discordant emotions. Skin conductance responses (SCRs) and heart rates (HRs) were measured during a classic Stroop task and one in which the emotions conveyed by lexicosemantic content and prosody were congruent or incongruent. The participants’ task was to identify the emotion conveyed by lexicosemantic content or prosody. No relationship was observed between HR and congruence. SCR was higher during incongruent than during congruent conditions of the experimental task (as well as in the classic Stroop task), but no difference in SCR was observed in a comparison between congruence effects during lexicosemantic emotion identification and those during prosodic emotion identification. It is concluded that incongruence between lexicosemantic and prosodic emotion does cause notable cognitive conflict. Functional neuroanatomic implications are discussed.  相似文献   
129.
130.
Estes and Maddox (2002) suggested that the word frequency mirror effect in episodic recognition memory might be due to word likeness rather than to the frequency of experience with a word per se. We examined their suggestion using a factorial manipulation of frequency and neighborhood density, a measure used in lexical memory research to measure orthographic word likeness. For study with no specified task, main effects of density and frequency were in the mirror order, confirming the hypothesized mirror effect of word likeness but not its role in producing the frequency mirror effect. Lexical decision study increased the size of both mirror effects, even though the density manipulation had a negligible effect on lexical decision performance for words. Post hoc analyses showed that neither mirror effect could be explained by differences in lower order measures of word likeness (letter and bigram frequency). The joint orders of frequency and density results were mirrored across new and old conditions in accordance with attention likelihood theory (ALT), but density effects on z-ROC slope suggest that ALT may require extension to accommodate the effect of word likeness on response confidence.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号