全文获取类型
收费全文 | 48762篇 |
免费 | 947篇 |
国内免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
49715篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 533篇 |
2018年 | 761篇 |
2017年 | 742篇 |
2016年 | 831篇 |
2015年 | 612篇 |
2014年 | 744篇 |
2013年 | 4731篇 |
2012年 | 1297篇 |
2011年 | 1347篇 |
2010年 | 885篇 |
2009年 | 857篇 |
2008年 | 1298篇 |
2007年 | 1171篇 |
2006年 | 1110篇 |
2005年 | 993篇 |
2004年 | 980篇 |
2003年 | 984篇 |
2002年 | 992篇 |
2001年 | 1375篇 |
2000年 | 1291篇 |
1999年 | 1043篇 |
1998年 | 569篇 |
1997年 | 537篇 |
1996年 | 636篇 |
1995年 | 627篇 |
1994年 | 590篇 |
1993年 | 584篇 |
1992年 | 1041篇 |
1991年 | 918篇 |
1990年 | 930篇 |
1989年 | 817篇 |
1988年 | 811篇 |
1987年 | 805篇 |
1986年 | 793篇 |
1985年 | 809篇 |
1984年 | 766篇 |
1983年 | 671篇 |
1982年 | 551篇 |
1981年 | 560篇 |
1980年 | 514篇 |
1979年 | 689篇 |
1978年 | 586篇 |
1976年 | 531篇 |
1975年 | 650篇 |
1974年 | 664篇 |
1973年 | 678篇 |
1972年 | 627篇 |
1971年 | 558篇 |
1970年 | 499篇 |
1968年 | 598篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
921.
Yakov M. Epstein Richard Teitelbaum Robert A. Karlin Sally Katz John R. Aiello 《Journal of applied social psychology》1981,11(3):259-267
Studies of crowding in various settings have shown that individuals are aroused and frequently suffer from ill health. Several studies have shown that it is possible to successfully intervene in residential, shopping, and classroom settings to reduce the negative effects of crowding. The present laboratory experiment assessed the effectiveness of two tactics commonly used by passengers in crowded mass transit settings. Results indicated that the two tactics, reading a newspaper or talking to others, failed to reduce arousal. An explanation for the failure of these tactics is offered. 相似文献
922.
John J Godfrey A.K. Syrdal-Lasky Kathleen K Millay Carol M Knox 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1981,32(3):401-424
Several researchers who have compared the performance of dyslexic and normal-reading children on a variety of different tasks have suggested that dyslexic children may have subtle deficits in the phonemic analysis of spoken as well as written language. Thus it is of interest to know how children who have extraordinary difficulty learning to read can perform explicity auditory-phonetic tasks. Seventeen dyslexic children (10 years of age) and a group of 17 controls were administered tests of identification and discrimination of synthesized voiced stop consonants differing in place of articulation. These were tests of the type used to study categorical perception in adults, adapted for use with young children. Significant differences between dyslexics and controls were found in both kinds of tasks; the pattern of identification and discrimination differences suggests an inconsistency in the dyslexics' phonetic classification of auditory cues. A significant relationship was found between reading level and speech discrimination. 相似文献
923.
Several recent experiments have demonstrated that modulation of the facial expressive response is accompanied by changes in autonomic arousal and subjective response to painful stimuli. The present study asked whether facial self-regulation may also bring about changes in covert vicarious emotional experience. Three groups of subjects were exposed to a videotaped model displaying intermittent pain to shock in a differential vicarious autonomic conditioning paradigm. Subjects in the inhibit and amplify groups were asked, respectively, (a) to inhibit their facial muscles or (b) to pose a facial response of pain when the model was shocked. It was predicted that the inhibit group would show less autonomic arousal to the model's expressive display (empathy) and less conditioning (as measured by skin conductance and heart rate change), and the amplify group more empathy and conditioning, than a third group who was given no facial instruction. In fact, the amplify group showed more skin conductance arousal, heart rate acceleration, and activity in response to the model's expressive display of pain than did the other two groups (which were not different from each other), but no more autonomic or facial conditioning. The overall pattern of physiological data is interpreted as generally supportive of a facial feedback theory of emotion: where significant between-groups' differences were obtained in facial activity, as in vicarious instigation, autonomic arousal differences also emerged; where no expressive differences were obtained, as in vicarious conditioning, no differences in autonomic arousal were found. 相似文献
924.
Robert H. Dolliver Ph.D. 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》1981,12(1):53-59
Personality and psychotherapy theorists choose certain elements to emphasize in their theories. A major, though usually unrecognized, source for such elements is the personal life of the theorist. Examples are drawn from nine theorists in the areas of family experiences, personal relationships, special experiences, and significant clients. The overall thesis is that an eclectic orientation to psychotherapy may be most appropriate since: (a) all theories emphasize certain elements (and ignore others), which is (b) often aided by (and limited by) the theorist's own life and experiences. Thus, if the client's personal life resembles the theorist's personal life, then the emphases developed by that theorist are most likely to be helpful to that client. 相似文献
925.
John Gibbon 《Journal of mathematical psychology》1981,24(1):58-87
The Psychometric Bisection Function for time relates the discriminability of intermediate duration stimuli to a short and long training duration. Bisection Functions for animals (R. M. Church & M. Z. Deluty, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1977, 3, 216–228) confirm Weber's Law and also show indifference between short and long reports at the geometric mean of the training durations. Two discrimination processes are studied which, in combination with different constructions of the subjective time scale, result in Bisection Functions which differ in form and location. The two discrimination processes use a likelihood ratio rule or a similarity rule to compare intermediate durations to the training durations. These rules in combination with two different constructions of the subjective time scale result in four models which conform to Weber's Law. For one of the scales subjective time is a power function of real time with the scalar property on variance (Scalar Timing). For the other, subjective time is a logarithmic function of real time with constant variance (Log Timing). Both Log and Scalar Timing assume normality on the subjective scale. Only three of these models also entail the geometric mean at the indifference point. The exception is Scalar Timing with the likelihood ratio discrimination rule. This model entails indifference at approximately the harmonic mean of the training stimuli. Variants of the remaining three models differ theoretically but alternatives are difficult to discriminate empirically. A contrast is provided by a Poisson Timing subjective scale in which variance increases directly with the mean. This scale results in indifference at the geometric mean for both discrimination rules but violates Weber's Law in both cases. 相似文献
926.
927.
A system for monitoring blood pressure or other physiological variables in the unanesthetized and chronically instrumented dog is described. An indwelling catheter attaches to a pressure transducer affixed to the underside of a leather shield, which snaps onto a custom-fitted leather vest worn by the dog. The transducer cable ascends vertically through a hollow, flexible, stainless steel tether, which is attached at the lower end to the leather shield and at the upper end to a fluid and electrical swivel. The swivel is attached to an overhead counterbalanced boom assembly, which moves in concert with the dog, maintaining a constant minimal tension on the tether. The boom assembly is attached to a wall plate via a universal joint approximately 7 ft above floor level. Dogs can be maintained comfortably in this system 24 h/day over periods of months, with freedom of posture and movement, including rotational behavior. The system is economical to construct and provides versatile stimulation and record-ing capability and reliable long-term performance. 相似文献
928.
Harold P. Edman John H. Greist Marjorie H. Klein James W. Jefferson Carl Getto 《Behavior research methods》1981,13(4):393-398
The use of computers in psychiatry and psychology is reviewed. It is noted that computers are already being used successfully for consultation, interviewing, and continuing education. Issues related to the usage of computers in mental health are discussed. Guidelines for future work in the area are suggested. 相似文献
929.
Irwin D. J. Bross 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》1981,2(2):145-153
Alarmed by Three Mile Island, Love Canal, and other disastrous deployments of new technologies, the public is demanding more voice in the decision-making on technology. To make this possible, it is proposed that metatechnology be evolved that will provide a practical technology for the safe, effective, and economical use of technology. A case history of a specific metatechnology is presented. Here the metatechnology enabled a realistic balancing of the benefits of mass screening of women by mammography against the hazards from the X-rays. By showing that the screening of women under 50 was counterproductive, this metatechnology helped to ban such screening in a program involving a quarter of a million women. The strategic mistake in setting up this and other technological programs was that the decisions were made by the technologists. They benefited the technologists but were not beneficial to the women. One of the potentials of metatechnology is that in its fully computerized versions it could be used directly by the public for decision-making on the deployment of technologies. 相似文献
930.