全文获取类型
收费全文 | 53234篇 |
免费 | 1060篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
54299篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 548篇 |
2019年 | 650篇 |
2018年 | 915篇 |
2017年 | 903篇 |
2016年 | 931篇 |
2015年 | 644篇 |
2014年 | 852篇 |
2013年 | 3804篇 |
2012年 | 1520篇 |
2011年 | 1646篇 |
2010年 | 947篇 |
2009年 | 1012篇 |
2008年 | 1432篇 |
2007年 | 1416篇 |
2006年 | 1323篇 |
2005年 | 1161篇 |
2004年 | 1145篇 |
2003年 | 1074篇 |
2002年 | 1072篇 |
2001年 | 1676篇 |
2000年 | 1595篇 |
1999年 | 1226篇 |
1998年 | 601篇 |
1997年 | 510篇 |
1992年 | 976篇 |
1991年 | 915篇 |
1990年 | 942篇 |
1989年 | 895篇 |
1988年 | 872篇 |
1987年 | 813篇 |
1986年 | 853篇 |
1985年 | 946篇 |
1984年 | 752篇 |
1983年 | 689篇 |
1982年 | 555篇 |
1981年 | 517篇 |
1979年 | 800篇 |
1978年 | 600篇 |
1977年 | 494篇 |
1976年 | 529篇 |
1975年 | 699篇 |
1974年 | 793篇 |
1973年 | 819篇 |
1972年 | 646篇 |
1971年 | 592篇 |
1970年 | 564篇 |
1969年 | 599篇 |
1968年 | 743篇 |
1967年 | 644篇 |
1966年 | 658篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
Previous work with the Geriatric Hopelessness Scale (GHS; Fry, 1984, 1986) suggests that it may be a useful measure of pessimism for a variety of domains among elderly persons. One hundred seven community-residing aged persons were individually administered the GHS and a variety of personality and cognitive functioning measures in an effort to further refine and validate the GHS. Results suggest very modest internal consistency and a poorly defined factor structure for the GHS, in contrast to earlier findings. Although the GHS failed to differentiate elderly persons by age, it was nonetheless related to self-esteem, depression, self-rated health, internality, morale, and life satisfaction. Taken together, these data indicate that the research and clinical utility of the GHS with community-residing aged persons may be more limited than previously believed. 相似文献
992.
Preferences for separating or combining events 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
993.
P K Lunt 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1991,61(1):26-34
Two studies examined the way people represent the causes of loneliness as a semantic space and as a causal network. In a multidimensional scaling task, the results of Michela, Peplau, and Weeks (1982) were replicated and the causes wee classified according to locus and stability. In a 2nd study, 44 people judged the likelihood that these causes were interconnected. From these data, a causal network or perceived causal structure (Kelley, 1983) was derived by means of network analysis. The structure revealed a complex lay theory of loneliness in which personality influenced group behavior, which in turn influenced psychological states and beliefs. States and beliefs were also affected by physical features and situational factors. 相似文献
994.
Confusing one person with another: what errors reveal about the elementary forms of social relations 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Seven studies investigated the cognitive structure of social relationships exhibited in the patterns of substitutions that occur when people confuse a person with another. The studies investigated natural errors in which people called a familiar person by the wrong name, misremembered with whom they had interacted, or mistakenly directed an action at an inappropriate person. These studies tested the relational-models theory of A. P. Fiske (1990b, 1991) that people use 4 basic models for social relationships. All 7 studies provide support for the theory; Ss tend to confuse people with whom they interact in the same basic relationship mode. In addition, Ss confuse people of the same gender. Other factors (age, race, role term, similarity of names) generally have smaller, less reliable effects, indicating that the 4 elementary modes of relationships are among the most salient schemata in everyday social cognition. 相似文献
995.
996.
C K Sigelman J L Howell D P Cornell J D Cutright J C Dewey 《The Journal of social psychology》1991,131(1):45-56
We investigated the operation of courtesy stigma with American male college students who reacted to a fictitious male student described as gay, rooming by choice with a gay male student, involuntarily assigned to room with a gay, or rooming with a male heterosexual. Among respondents who expressed strong intolerance of gays, the voluntary associate of a gay was perceived as having homosexual tendencies and as possessing the same stereotyped personality traits attributed to a gay. No such courtesy stigma was attached to the involuntary associate of a gay by these respondents. Relatively tolerant respondents engaged in no courtesy stigmatization at all. Thus, courtesy stigmatization occurred only under circumscribed conditions and appeared to depend more on the tendency of highly intolerant individuals to infer that a male student who apparently liked a gay individual was himself gay than on a motivation to maintain cognitive consistency. 相似文献
997.
This study examines the behavior of the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-II (MCMI-II) in the face of various response styles and biases. The profiles and validity configurations of eight different test-taking styles were analyzed. Four hundred MCMI-II inventories (50 for each of the following categories) were administered or generated to produce the following: (a) normal endorsement by subjects, (b) fake good for administrative reasons, (c) fake good for clinical reasons, (d) fake bad administratively, (e) fake bad clinically, (f) 50% true/50% false computer generated, (g) 95% true computer generated, and (h) 95% false computer generated. Good statistical and clinically relevant separation of the profiles was found for normal, fake good, fake bad, and the randomly generated profiles with 44% of the variance predicted. The percentage of profiles identified by validity scales, however, was modest. 相似文献
998.
C J Hallinan E F Pierce J E Evans J D DeGrenier F F Andres 《Perceptual and motor skills》1991,72(1):123-130
This study was done to examine the relationship between sex and perception of body image among athletes and nonathletes. A total of 211 men and women athletes and nonathletes completed a questionnaire which asked them to identify from a nine-figure body-silhouette scale their current image and the image that they thought was most ideal. t tests showed no significant differences for men based upon athletic participation and that both athletes and nonathletes were satisfied with their body image. However, when comparing current image and ideal image, significant differences were found for women athletes and nonathletes. These data support previous research documenting women's dissatisfaction with their body images and show athletic participation is not associated with this perception. 相似文献
999.
Formant frequency characteristics of children, young adult, and aged female speakers 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Previous literature indicates that a difference may exist between formant frequencies (F1 and F2) of children, young adult, and elderly speakers. The purpose of this study was to compare F1 and F2 of 3 young, 6 young adult, and 3 elderly female speakers for the /i/, /ae/, /u/, and /a/ vowels. Analysis indicates a trend towards vowel reduction across the life span. These findings support previous research regarding age-associated acoustic changes as well as support for the possible anatomical and physiological alterations which may influence such changes. 相似文献
1000.
The relationship between visuospatial ability and size accuracy in perception was assessed in 69 normal college females. In general, correlations indicated small associations between visuospatial defects and size overestimation and little relationship between visuospatial ability and level of bulimic disturbance. Implications for research on the size overestimation of body image are addressed. 相似文献