首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   9995篇
  免费   288篇
  国内免费   3篇
  10286篇
  2023年   44篇
  2021年   58篇
  2020年   116篇
  2019年   127篇
  2018年   160篇
  2017年   185篇
  2016年   182篇
  2015年   287篇
  2014年   287篇
  2013年   1249篇
  2012年   439篇
  2011年   404篇
  2010年   348篇
  2009年   374篇
  2008年   435篇
  2007年   380篇
  2006年   352篇
  2005年   331篇
  2004年   308篇
  2003年   267篇
  2002年   238篇
  2001年   117篇
  2000年   108篇
  1999年   118篇
  1998年   125篇
  1997年   91篇
  1996年   107篇
  1995年   135篇
  1994年   108篇
  1993年   100篇
  1992年   93篇
  1991年   113篇
  1990年   93篇
  1989年   110篇
  1988年   104篇
  1987年   80篇
  1986年   83篇
  1985年   99篇
  1984年   103篇
  1983年   92篇
  1982年   101篇
  1981年   94篇
  1980年   110篇
  1979年   84篇
  1978年   103篇
  1977年   107篇
  1976年   117篇
  1975年   107篇
  1974年   102篇
  1973年   63篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
Tibbetts  Paul 《Synthese》2004,141(2):247-276
Synthese - The concept of voluntary motor control(VMC) frequently appears in the neuroscientific literature, specifically in the context of cortically-mediated, intentional motor actions. For...  相似文献   
12.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
13.
14.
ABSTRACT

Hegel's Phenomenology of Spirit provides a fascinating picture of individual minds caught up in “recognitive” relations so as to constitute a realm—“spirit”—which, while necessarily embedded in nature, is not reducible to it. In this essay I suggest a contemporary path for developing Hegel's suggestive ideas in a way that broadly conforms to the demands of his own system, such that one moves from logic to a philosophy of mind. Hence I draw on Hegel's “subjective logic”, understood in the light of modern modal logic, in an attempt to model the way minds might be thought as connected by way of shared intentional contents. Here, we should not be surprised at some of the parallels that emerge between the approaches of Hegel and the modal logician Arthur Prior, as Prior had testified to the influence of his teacher, John N. Findlay, who himself had strong Hegelian leanings. In the final section, Robert Stalnaker's version of possible-world semantics is suggested as a framework within which Hegel's recognitive account of the mind might be understood.  相似文献   
15.
16.
17.
A scale assessing generalized false self‐perceptions (Perceptions of False Self, POFS) was developed and tested across three studies involving a total of 331 adolescents (11–16 years). In Study 1, interviews were conducted to develop items for the scale. In Study 2, psychometric techniques were used to derive a scale composed of 16 items. Study 2 also assessed the validity of the scale: depressive symptoms were found to increase POFS across 10 weeks, whereas false self‐perceptions had a reciprocal effect on anxiety. In Study 3, the convergent validity of the POFS scale was established. Overall, the evidence suggests that the POFS scale is a reliable and valid measure of generalized false self‐perceptions.  相似文献   
18.
19.
20.
有两个问题常常被人们提及。第一个是为什么宗教可以持续至今?第二个是我们是否可以创造出世俗人文的替代品。对于为什么宗教可以一直延续这个问题,众说纷纭。  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号