全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4549篇 |
免费 | 69篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
4621篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 29篇 |
2019年 | 45篇 |
2018年 | 31篇 |
2017年 | 45篇 |
2016年 | 54篇 |
2015年 | 165篇 |
2014年 | 157篇 |
2013年 | 570篇 |
2012年 | 214篇 |
2011年 | 176篇 |
2010年 | 226篇 |
2009年 | 202篇 |
2008年 | 196篇 |
2007年 | 155篇 |
2006年 | 144篇 |
2005年 | 121篇 |
2004年 | 100篇 |
2003年 | 75篇 |
2002年 | 59篇 |
2001年 | 41篇 |
2000年 | 39篇 |
1999年 | 35篇 |
1997年 | 30篇 |
1996年 | 40篇 |
1995年 | 69篇 |
1994年 | 43篇 |
1993年 | 48篇 |
1992年 | 42篇 |
1991年 | 58篇 |
1990年 | 57篇 |
1989年 | 58篇 |
1988年 | 65篇 |
1987年 | 49篇 |
1986年 | 40篇 |
1985年 | 50篇 |
1984年 | 53篇 |
1983年 | 40篇 |
1982年 | 52篇 |
1981年 | 44篇 |
1980年 | 68篇 |
1979年 | 64篇 |
1978年 | 59篇 |
1977年 | 71篇 |
1976年 | 79篇 |
1975年 | 73篇 |
1974年 | 68篇 |
1973年 | 35篇 |
1938年 | 31篇 |
1937年 | 31篇 |
1936年 | 33篇 |
排序方式: 共有4621条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
Gordon W. Russell Sherry L. Di Lullo Dany Di Lullo 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》1988,7(4):312-321
Males were randomly assigned to view either (1) a film clip featuring hockey fights or (2) a film of nonaggressive hockey
action or (3) a no-film control condition after having first been angered or treated politely by an experimental confederate.
The dependent variable was represented by a measure of aggressive mood and a behavioral measure of retaliatory aggression.
Analyses revealed that both angered and nonangered subjects exhibited an increase in aggressive mood following exposure to
the fight film. However, the analysis involving retaliatory aggression against the confederate yielded an anger x film interaction.
While angered subjects were more aggressive than nonangered, only angered subjects retaliated against the confederate after
viewing the fight film. The results were discussed in terms of Berkowitz’s (1974) aggressive cue theory.
A version of this paper was presented at the meeting of the North American Society for the Sociology of Sport, Edmonton, Canada,
November 1987. 相似文献
12.
Gordon Halliday Ph.D. 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》1992,22(3):173-181
The concept of developmental amnesia is proposed to describe the impairment of normal memories during a person's school age life. One hundred psychotherapy patients were interviewed and the age of earliest memories ranged from 1–12 years, with a mode of 5 years. Memory was continuous for 53% of patients by 1st grade, while 18% reported life memory was still episodic at age 18 or later. Parental alcohol problems were associated with delay of continuous memories. 相似文献
13.
This study explored patterns emerging in videotaped sessions of group psychotherapy with seven outpatients of a large mental health facility. The 22 sessions spanned the 12th to 27th months of the group's existence. Group members had long histories (25–50 years) of symptoms characteristic of schizophrenia. Each session was rated on the Group Environment Scale. One session from a group in its earlier stages was also rated on the same instrument to give some indication of baseline patterns. Analyses of changes across the 15 months were not statistically significant. However, some GES subscales, including Cohesion, Independence, and Self-Discovery, showed relatively high mean levels across the 15-month period. These elevations are discussed as suggesting the codevelopment of orientations that have sometimes been viewed as incompatible and unattainable for schizophrenics. Interpreted clinically, the GES patterns help to elaborate recent findings about the relatively good success of long-term group therapy with such populations. The study, although merely suggestive, illustrates the potential for gleaning solid and clinically useful data from group psychotherapy research.The authors would like to thank Drs. Jill Rierdan, Nina Fieldsteel, Joseph Christy, and an anonymous reviewer for their suggestions, Kendra Bryant for serving as the reliability rater, and Janet Eltinge for her skilled videotaping. 相似文献
14.
Explanations of context effects in the Reicher-Wheeler task and the letter-identification task appeal to word-based processing, yet these tasks provide no explicit measure of word processing. An experiment is reported which was designed to investigate the use of transfer in the word-identification task as a measure of word-based processing in letter-identification tasks. It was found that encoding manipulations that determined whether a word-superiority effect was or was not found in a letter-identification task (e.g., Thompson & Massaro, 1973) also determined whether transfer was or was not found in a subsequent word-identification task. The results of the experiment are discussed in terms of the utility of using transfer experiments as converging evidence about the presence and/or absence of processes that cannot be directly measured in other experimental paradigms. 相似文献
15.
This study examined the ability of both an overt integrity test and a personality-based measure of delinquency to predict on-the-job theft and counterproductivity. A heterogeneous sample of 105 employees anonymously completed both tests and a criterion checklist that included a 9-item theft subscale and a 12-item counterproductivity subscale. Results indicated that the overt integrity test was a superior predictor of both on-the-job theft and drug abuse compared to the personality-based measure. The overt integrity test was also a slightly better predictor of general counterproductivity. These findings and their implications are discussed. 相似文献
16.
17.
18.
19.
The Jenkins Activity Survey (JAS) and the Framingham Type A Scale (FTAS) have been found to have different psychological correlates. The JAS is typically unrelated to anxiety, while the FTAS is consistently correlated with trait anxiety. The present study of 200 male and female college students clarifies further the psychological heterogeneity of these measures. Enhanced motivation to control the environment has been suggested as an underlying psychological component of Type A behavior (Glass, 1977). Dysphoric emotions and the perception of more environmental stresses, on the other hand, are typical of neuroticism or negative affectivity (Watson & Clark, 1984). Although the results indicate that the JAS and FTAS are highly correlated, the JAS is more closely related to control motivation than is the FTAS. Further, compared to the JAS, the FTAS is more closely correlated with dysphoric emotions and perceived daily stresses. Thus, the results indicate that the JAS is closely correlated with core psychological components of the Type A pattern, while the FTAS has a relatively unique association with general emotional distress. Such differences may account for the fact that these measures are related to different disease end points. 相似文献
20.
Rats were given backward pairings of a tone (CS) and shock (US) and were then tested for response to the CS in a lick suppression test. Animals given a cuing or reminder treatment prior to the test exhibited enhanced suppression in the presence of the tone relative to a variety of control conditions. The cue-induced suppression enhancement did not appear to result from sensitization or short-term motivational changes nor did it seem to depend on second-order conditioning of the test context. The effect appeared more robust as the number of backward conditioning trials preceding the cue increased. The results were discussed in terms of current explanations of backward conditioning effects. 相似文献