全文获取类型
收费全文 | 27972篇 |
免费 | 1015篇 |
国内免费 | 14篇 |
专业分类
29001篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 299篇 |
2018年 | 420篇 |
2017年 | 460篇 |
2016年 | 503篇 |
2015年 | 504篇 |
2014年 | 530篇 |
2013年 | 2489篇 |
2012年 | 873篇 |
2011年 | 854篇 |
2010年 | 631篇 |
2009年 | 617篇 |
2008年 | 752篇 |
2007年 | 735篇 |
2006年 | 657篇 |
2005年 | 601篇 |
2004年 | 550篇 |
2003年 | 538篇 |
2002年 | 548篇 |
2001年 | 800篇 |
2000年 | 767篇 |
1999年 | 593篇 |
1996年 | 287篇 |
1995年 | 277篇 |
1992年 | 499篇 |
1991年 | 537篇 |
1990年 | 502篇 |
1989年 | 478篇 |
1988年 | 499篇 |
1987年 | 449篇 |
1986年 | 452篇 |
1985年 | 448篇 |
1984年 | 396篇 |
1983年 | 342篇 |
1982年 | 295篇 |
1981年 | 291篇 |
1980年 | 269篇 |
1979年 | 435篇 |
1978年 | 337篇 |
1977年 | 316篇 |
1976年 | 303篇 |
1975年 | 374篇 |
1974年 | 444篇 |
1973年 | 433篇 |
1972年 | 317篇 |
1971年 | 307篇 |
1970年 | 302篇 |
1969年 | 274篇 |
1968年 | 380篇 |
1967年 | 337篇 |
1966年 | 321篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
111.
B P McCloskey R L Klatzky J W Pellegrino 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1992,18(4):948-961
Previous research (Klatzky et al., 1989) shows that the time required to make sensible/nonsensible judgments about an action-object phrase (e.g., "rub your stomach") is reliably faster when the phrase is preceded by a cure representing a specific prototypical hand shape (vs. a neutral cue). The current experiments investigated the effects of preparing for an alternate task (finger tapping vs. syllable vocalization) on facilitatory priming of sensibility judgments. Preparation for finger tapping reduced the magnitude of the priming effect more than preparation for vocalization, suggesting that resources accessed during semantic processing of action-object phrases are also used during manual response preparation. The results support the existence of a system representing manual actions that is limited in the number of activities that can be represented at one time and that is not so general that it represents manual and vocal tract movements. 相似文献
112.
Gender differences in children's play activities, preferred school subjects, and occupational goals were examined in relation to such parental variables as toy-giving and chore assignment. Subjects were 245 children in grades K, 3, 6, and 8. Subjects completed questionnaires before and after Christmas asking what gifts they wanted, asked for, received, and liked best. They also were asked to name their friends and play activities, favorite and least-liked school subjects, occupational aspirations, and chores at home. Children generally wanted, asked for, received, and most-liked gender-typical toys. They were less likely to receive requested gender-atypical toys. Children's preferred activities, job aspirations and assigned chores were along gender-typical lines. Girls preferred masculine toys and jobs more than boys preferred feminine ones. With increasing age, both girls and boys increasingly preferred masculine toys and male friends. No gender differences in favorite or least-liked school subjects were found. School subject preference was related to gender-typing of occupational choice for girls but not boys.An earlier version of this paper was presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Anaheim, California, 1983. 相似文献
113.
This study explored patterns emerging in videotaped sessions of group psychotherapy with seven outpatients of a large mental health facility. The 22 sessions spanned the 12th to 27th months of the group's existence. Group members had long histories (25–50 years) of symptoms characteristic of schizophrenia. Each session was rated on the Group Environment Scale. One session from a group in its earlier stages was also rated on the same instrument to give some indication of baseline patterns. Analyses of changes across the 15 months were not statistically significant. However, some GES subscales, including Cohesion, Independence, and Self-Discovery, showed relatively high mean levels across the 15-month period. These elevations are discussed as suggesting the codevelopment of orientations that have sometimes been viewed as incompatible and unattainable for schizophrenics. Interpreted clinically, the GES patterns help to elaborate recent findings about the relatively good success of long-term group therapy with such populations. The study, although merely suggestive, illustrates the potential for gleaning solid and clinically useful data from group psychotherapy research.The authors would like to thank Drs. Jill Rierdan, Nina Fieldsteel, Joseph Christy, and an anonymous reviewer for their suggestions, Kendra Bryant for serving as the reliability rater, and Janet Eltinge for her skilled videotaping. 相似文献
114.
Eighty younger (less than 50 years, M = 28 years) and 80 older (more than 50 years, M = 69 years) Type A and Type B Ss were evaluated for Type A behavior pattern using the Structured Interview (SI) and given personality tests for anxiety, depression, anger, aggression, hostility, and anger-in-anger-out. Ss also underwent an emotion induction procedure. Videotapes of the emotion induction procedure (N = 160) and the SI (N = 80) were coded for facial expression of emotion. Type As did not differ from Bs on anxiety or depression but did on anger and aggression. Type As showed anger inhibition and anger bound to shame, as predicted by emotion socialization theory. The greatest number of differential effects were observed between age groups. Older individuals, in general, were more emotionally expressive than younger Ss across a range of emotions. Women appeared more conflicted about anger expression than men, and Type A women more so than Type A men. 相似文献
115.
Older adults' life satisfaction can be better understood in light of their personal goals. This study of 179 elders examined (a) how goals correlate with satisfaction, (b) whether elders maintain satisfaction by accommodating goals to past losses, and (c) how correlations between satisfaction and key predictors differ among groups with different goals. Satisfaction was related positively to social maintenance and energetic life-style goals and negatively to concerns for improvement, disengagement, stability, and reduced activity. Past losses were correlated with current goals but not with satisfaction, consistent with the notion of accommodation. A cluster analysis identified 5 patterns of goals: high demand, age prescribed, self-focused, socially engaged, and low demand. Correlations between satisfaction and other predictors differed by cluster, suggesting that the determinants of elders' satisfaction depend on personal goals. 相似文献
116.
James B. Freeman 《Argumentation》1992,6(2):219-275
117.
118.
119.
John W. Schuster Ed.D. Ann K. Griffen B.S. Mark Wolery Ph.D. 《Journal of Behavioral Education》1992,2(3):305-325
Simultaneous prompting (a type of antecedent prompt and test procedure) and constant time delay were compared with four students with moderate mental retardation learning expressive sight words. A parallel treatments design across word sets and replicated across students was used. For acquisition, the simultaneous prompting procedures required fewer trials, sessions, and training time to criterion and resulted in fewer student errors during daily probe and training sessions. However, maintenance data indicated mixed results across the two procedures. Reliability data (both dependent and independent variables) revealed no differences between the two procedures in terms of the teacher's accuracy in recording student responses and implementing each procedure. Future research issues are discussed. 相似文献
120.