首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   165篇
  免费   5篇
  2023年   1篇
  2020年   3篇
  2019年   2篇
  2018年   4篇
  2017年   5篇
  2016年   5篇
  2015年   1篇
  2014年   4篇
  2013年   13篇
  2012年   5篇
  2011年   3篇
  2010年   3篇
  2009年   17篇
  2008年   6篇
  2007年   7篇
  2006年   3篇
  2005年   3篇
  2004年   5篇
  2003年   8篇
  2002年   11篇
  2001年   3篇
  2000年   2篇
  1999年   3篇
  1998年   4篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   5篇
  1994年   3篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   4篇
  1990年   3篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   1篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   2篇
  1981年   2篇
  1979年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   3篇
  1974年   3篇
  1973年   1篇
  1972年   2篇
  1959年   2篇
  1955年   1篇
排序方式: 共有170条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Previous detection performances obtained in a multi-varied contrast environment showed that observers tend to report significantly less the occurrence of the relatively lower contrast targets and significantly more the occurrence of the higher contrast ones than in conditions where each of these stimuli was tested in isolation (Gorea & Sagi, 2000). This criterion shift phenomenon is a form of contrast-dependent extinction in normal observers. To add more ground to the analogy between this type of “natural” extinction and the stroke-related one, we present a series of experiments where identical targets are displayed (1) at the same eccentricity but in opposite hemifields (along both the horizontal and vertical meridians), (2) at different eccentricities, and (3) where different spatial frequency targets of equal contrasts are displayed at the same eccentricity. Hemifield, eccentricity, and spatial frequency manipulations were intended to entail sensitivity differences for identical contrast stimuli. Some observers showed up to 3 and 1.5 d'-units differences between their left and right and between their upper and lower hemifields, respectively. Although observers may show significant response biases even for isolated stimuli as well as for equally visible ones presented together, it is always the case that they use higher response criteria (extinction) for the less visible targets and lower response criteria for the more visible ones (counter-extinction) when these stimuli are mixed in one experimental block. The observed criterion shifts translate into as much as three times more “Not seen” responses for the less visible targets and into about 1.3 times more “Seen” responses for the more visible ones. Taken together, the present data support the notion that at least some forms of extinction (and perhaps neglect) are contingent on relative sensory/perceptual impairments and that they reflect more or less drastic forms of criterion/decision shifts.  相似文献   
62.
63.
Abstract

It is assumed that psychological stress develops in personally relevant events when situational demands tax or exceed coping resources (i.e., when coping options are severely limited). Despite the central role of this proposition in defining stress, it has been little investigated due to conceptual overlap in definitions of primary and secondary appraisals. In the present work, the interaction between primary and secondary appraisals was used in order to test this proposition, employing non-overlapping definitions of appraisal components. Appraisals were manipulated independently, using written scenarios of stressful occupational episodes, each with three levels of personal stakes and three levels of coping options. Results indicated that the appraisal factors exerted main, but not interaction, effects on negative and positive moods. This means that emotional response increases in proportion to the level of personal stakes in the encounter, with coping potential acting as independent predictor, rather than moderator variable. Contrary to expectations, therefore, emotional distress might develop under conditions where situational demands fall short of exerting all the available resources for coping. The theoretical implications of these results are discussed.  相似文献   
64.
The CuPS (Culture × Person × Situation) approach attempts to jointly consider culture and individual differences, without treating either as noise and without reducing one to the other. Culture is important because it helps define psychological situations and create meaningful clusters of behavior according to particular logics. Individual differences are important because individuals vary in the extent to which they endorse or reject a culture's ideals. Further, because different cultures are organized by different logics, individual differences mean something different in each. Central to these studies are concepts of honor-related violence and individual worth as being inalienable versus socially conferred. We illustrate our argument with 2 experiments involving participants from honor, face, and dignity cultures. The studies showed that the same "type" of person who was most helpful, honest, and likely to behave with integrity in one culture was the "type" of person least likely to do so in another culture. We discuss how CuPS can provide a rudimentary but integrated approach to understanding both within- and between-culture variation.  相似文献   
65.
Adults use both bottom‐up sensory inputs and top‐down signals to generate predictions about future sensory inputs. Infants have also been shown to make predictions with simple stimuli and recent work has suggested top‐down processing is available early in infancy. However, it is unknown whether this indicates that top‐down prediction is an ability that is continuous across the lifespan or whether an infant's ability to predict is different from an adult's, qualitatively or quantitatively. We employed pupillometry to provide a direct comparison of prediction abilities across these disparate age groups. Pupil dilation response (PDR) was measured in 6‐month olds and adults as they completed an identical implicit learning task designed to help learn associations between sounds and pictures. We found significantly larger PDR for visual omission trials (i.e. trials that violated participants’ predictions without the presentation of new stimuli to control for bottom‐up signals) compared to visual present trials (i.e. trials that confirmed participants’ predictions) in both age groups. Furthermore, a computational learning model that is closely linked to prediction error (Rescorla‐Wagner model) demonstrated similar learning trajectories suggesting a continuity of predictive capacity and learning across the two age groups.  相似文献   
66.
67.
Gabbay  Dov  Shehtman  Valentin 《Studia Logica》2002,72(2):157-183
In this paper we improve the results of [2] by proving the product f.m.p. for the product of minimal n-modal and minimal n-temporal logic. For this case we modify the finite depth method introduced in [1]. The main result is applied to identify new fragments of classical first-order logic and of the equational theory of relation algebras, that are decidable and have the finite model property.  相似文献   
68.
My thanks to Jacob Joshua Ross for his helpful advice, and to an anonymous reviewer for detailed and useful comments on an earlier version of this essay.  相似文献   
69.
Introduction     
Gabbay  Dov  Pirri  Fiora 《Studia Logica》1997,59(2):147-148
  相似文献   
70.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号