全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2185篇 |
免费 | 20篇 |
专业分类
2205篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 30篇 |
2019年 | 32篇 |
2018年 | 54篇 |
2017年 | 55篇 |
2016年 | 43篇 |
2015年 | 43篇 |
2014年 | 48篇 |
2013年 | 204篇 |
2012年 | 86篇 |
2011年 | 86篇 |
2010年 | 45篇 |
2009年 | 55篇 |
2008年 | 76篇 |
2007年 | 75篇 |
2006年 | 88篇 |
2005年 | 62篇 |
2004年 | 58篇 |
2003年 | 62篇 |
2002年 | 62篇 |
2001年 | 37篇 |
2000年 | 36篇 |
1999年 | 48篇 |
1998年 | 35篇 |
1997年 | 34篇 |
1996年 | 45篇 |
1995年 | 29篇 |
1994年 | 33篇 |
1993年 | 37篇 |
1992年 | 23篇 |
1991年 | 23篇 |
1990年 | 28篇 |
1989年 | 23篇 |
1988年 | 24篇 |
1987年 | 30篇 |
1986年 | 23篇 |
1985年 | 26篇 |
1984年 | 25篇 |
1983年 | 27篇 |
1982年 | 41篇 |
1981年 | 30篇 |
1980年 | 27篇 |
1979年 | 32篇 |
1978年 | 23篇 |
1977年 | 24篇 |
1976年 | 26篇 |
1975年 | 24篇 |
1974年 | 16篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
1969年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有2205条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
181.
Douglas W. Portmore 《Philosophical Studies》2017,174(12):2953-2972
The performance of one option can entail the performance of another. For instance, I have the option of baking a pie as well as the option of baking, and baking a pie entails baking. Now, suppose that I have both reason to bake and reason to bake a pie. Which, if either, grounds the other? Do I have reason to bake in virtue of my having reason to perform some instance of baking, such as pie baking? Or do I have reason to bake a pie in virtue of my having reason to bake? Or does neither ground the other? Perhaps, the reason in each case is grounded in the fact that each option would itself have optimal consequences. The aim of this paper is to compare two alternative responses to this issue—omnism and maximalism—and to argue that the latter is more plausible. Omnism is the view that what grounds a reason for performing an option is always that it has some feature F (such as that of having optimal consequences). By contrast, maximalism holds that sometimes what grounds a reason for performing an option is not that it is itself F, but that it is entailed by some other option that is F. I’ll argue that maximalism is more plausible, for it avoids two critical problems that befall omnism. 相似文献
182.
Jamie L. Goldenberg Douglas P. Cooper Clay Routledge 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2011,47(2):443-448
From the perspective of terror management theory, awareness of death induces a need for validation of important values. Thus, for women who place a high value on their appearance (e.g., high self-objectifiers), mortality salience should increase positive reactions to objectifying experiences relative to women who do not highly value appearance. Two studies supported this hypothesis. Self-objectification moderated favorable reactions to objectifying stimuli (Study 1) and state self-esteem in response to an objectifying comment (Study 2) when women were primed with death. Together, the studies illustrate the complexity of reactions to objectification and, by highlighting conditions in which objectification serves a psychological function, help to explain the pervasiveness of the phenomena. 相似文献
183.
David M. Douglas 《Science and engineering ethics》2011,17(3):485-502
Software ownership allows the owner to restrict the distribution of software and to prevent others from reading the software’s
source code and building upon it. However, free software is released to users under software licenses that give them the right
to read the source code, modify it, reuse it, and distribute the software to others. Proponents of free software such as Richard
M. Stallman and Eben Moglen argue that the social disutility of software ownership is a sufficient justification for prohibiting
it. This social disutility includes the social instability of disregarding laws and agreements covering software use and distribution,
inequality of software access, and the inability to help others by sharing software with them. Here I consider these and other
social disutility claims against withholding specific software rights from users, in particular, the rights to read the source
code, duplicate, distribute, modify, imitate, and reuse portions of the software within new programs. I find that generally
while withholding these rights from software users does cause some degree of social disutility, only the rights to duplicate,
modify and imitate cannot legitimately be denied to users on this basis. The social disutility of withholding the rights to
distribute the software, read its source code and reuse portions of it in new programs is insufficient to prohibit software
owners from denying them to users. A compromise between the software owner and user can minimise the social disutility of
withholding these particular rights from users. However, the social disutility caused by software patents is sufficient for
rejecting such patents as they restrict the methods of reducing social disutility possible with other forms of software ownership. 相似文献
184.
McGrath RE Pogge DL Stokes JM Cragnolino A Zaccario M Hayman J Piacentini T Wayland-Smith D 《Assessment》2005,12(2):199-209
The extent to which the Comprehensive System for the Rorschach is reliably scored has been a topic of some controversy. Although several studies have concluded it can be scored reliably in research settings, little is known about its reliability in field settings. This study evaluated the reliability of both response-level codes and protocol-level scores among 84 adolescent psychiatric inpatients in a clinical setting. Rorschachs were originally administered and scored for clinical purposes. Among response codes, 87% demonstrated acceptable reliability(> .60), and most coefficients exceeded .80. Results were similar for protocol-level scores, with only one score demonstrating less than adequate reliability. The findings are consistent with previous evidence, indicating reliable scoring is possible even in field settings. 相似文献
185.
Schmitt DP Alcalay L Allik J Angleitner A Ault L Austers I Bennett KL Bianchi G Boholst F Borg Cunen MA Braeckman J Brainerd EG Caral LG Caron G Casullo MM Cunningham M Daibo I De Backer C De Souza E Diaz-Loving R Diniz G Durkin K Echegaray M Eremsoy E Euler HA Falzon R Fisher ML Foley D Fry DP Fry S Ghayur MA Golden DL Grammer K Grimaldi L Halberstadt J Haque S Herrera D Hertel J Hoffmann H Hooper D Hradilekova Z Hudek-Kene-evi J Jaafar J Jankauskaite M Kabangu-Stahel H Kardum I Khoury B 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2004,86(4):560-584
186.
Mothers' insightfulness, which involves understanding the motives underlying the child's behavior in a complete, open, and accepting way, was examined both at the beginning and at the end of a therapeutic preschool program for preschoolers using the Insightfulness Assessment (Oppenheim & Koren‐Karie, 2002). The goal of the study was to examine the links between improvement in mothers' insightfulness and reduction in children's behavior problems. Thirty‐two children referred to a therapeutic preschool program for a range of behavioral and emotional problems and their mothers participated in the study. Results showed that mothers who shifted from noninsightfulness before treatment to insightfulness after treatment had children whose behavior problems decreased whereas mothers who did not make such gains had children whose behavior problems increased. The social skills of all children improved during treatment. The possible mechanisms linking the gains of mothers and children are discussed within the framework of attachment theory. 相似文献
187.
David C. Houghton Maddison R. Franklin Michael P. Twohig Martin E. Franklin Scott N. Compton Angela M. Neal-Barnett Stephen M. Saunders Douglas W. Woods 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2016,38(3):505-513
Although photographic assessment has been found to be reliable in assessing hair loss in Trichotillomania, the validity of this method is unclear, particularly for gauging progress in treatment. The current study evaluated the psychometric properties of photographic assessment of change in Trichotillomania. Photographs showing hair loss of adults with Trichotillomania were taken before and after participating in a clinical trial for the condition. Undergraduate college students (N?=?211) rated treatment response according to the photos, and additional archival data on hair pulling severity and psychosocial health were retrieved from the clinical trial. Photographic assessment of change was found to possess fair reliability (ICC?=?0.53), acceptable criterion validity (r?=?0.51), good concurrent validity (r?=?0.30–0.36), and excellent incremental validity (ΔR 2?=?8.67, p?<?0.01). In addition, photographic measures were significantly correlated with change in quality of life (r?=?0.42), and thus could be considered an index of the social validity of Trichotillomania treatment. Gender of the photo rater and pulling topography affected the criterion validity of photographic assessment (partial η 2?=?0.05–0.11). Recommendations for improving photographic assessment and future directions for hair pulling research are discussed. 相似文献
188.
In a laboratory study, 318 student participants (148 male, 169 female, and one who did not report sex; M age 25.0, SD = 6.0) in introductory organizational behavior classes responded to scenarios in which performance appraisal resulted in either employee promotion or termination. Each scenario had varying levels of three procedural justice criteria for performance appraisal. For both promotion and termination outcomes, analysis showed that, as the number of criteria increased, perceptions of procedural fairness increased. A comparison between the two outcomes showed that perceptions of fairness were significantly stronger for the promotion outcome than for termination. 相似文献
189.
It is shown, with intuitionistic logic, that if every locallyconstant function from to has a property akinto constancy, then the fan theorem for -bars holds, and conversely. 相似文献
190.
F. Richard Ferraro Kim Christopherson Jason Douglas 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2006,25(2):132-143
University undergraduates were pre-screened for blood fearfulness (fear of blood, n = 25), spider fearfulness (fear of spiders, n = 30), or non-fearfulness (n = 23) and presented with blood, spider, neutral, positive, and pseudoword (pronouncable nonword, like flirp) stimuli in a
lexical decision task (LDT). Use of the LDT in phobic individuals may provide insight about how these individuals process,
store, and ultimately use information and, in turn, how processing this information affects their day-to-day activities. Words
were responded to faster than pseudowords. No group main or interaction effects emerged, and all groups responded faster and
more accurately to spider words then blood words and neutral words. Results suggest that the single-word lexical decision
task may not be sensitive in detecting lexical processing biases toward threat in these groups. Results also suggest that
paradigms that are based on high-level, semantic-conceptual information processing (like lexical decision) are not sensitive
enough to detect group differences in blood and spider phobias. Any array of perceptual and conceptual tasks taken together
may be needed to detect these differences. 相似文献