首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   33115篇
  免费   1411篇
  国内免费   25篇
  2020年   367篇
  2019年   450篇
  2018年   653篇
  2017年   625篇
  2016年   691篇
  2015年   465篇
  2014年   573篇
  2013年   2618篇
  2012年   1076篇
  2011年   1067篇
  2010年   631篇
  2009年   706篇
  2008年   913篇
  2007年   926篇
  2006年   838篇
  2005年   716篇
  2004年   703篇
  2003年   654篇
  2002年   676篇
  2001年   1086篇
  2000年   1092篇
  1999年   823篇
  1998年   358篇
  1997年   337篇
  1996年   328篇
  1995年   339篇
  1992年   649篇
  1991年   617篇
  1990年   594篇
  1989年   564篇
  1988年   562篇
  1987年   508篇
  1986年   509篇
  1985年   557篇
  1984年   458篇
  1983年   422篇
  1982年   345篇
  1979年   501篇
  1978年   348篇
  1976年   343篇
  1975年   383篇
  1974年   447篇
  1973年   476篇
  1972年   404篇
  1971年   348篇
  1970年   329篇
  1969年   350篇
  1968年   425篇
  1967年   376篇
  1966年   359篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 32 毫秒
51.
52.
53.
54.
Promises     
GRANT CK 《Mind》1949,58(231):359-366
  相似文献   
55.
56.
57.
In two experiments, right-handed men and women were tested for ear differences in report of dichotically presented digits, with their heads straight ahead, turned 90 degrees to the left, and turned 90 degrees to the right. In Experiment 1, head turn was controlled simply by asking the subjects to fixate an appropriately located point; a right-ear advantage occurred under all conditions of head turn among the men, but only in the head-straight condition among the women. In Experiment 2, head turn was controlled by having the subjects direct a flashlight attached to their heads toward the fixation point. This eliminated the right-ear advantage under all head conditions for the men, but for the women the right-ear advantage was, if anything, more pronounced when their heads were turned than when straight. These results suggest that auditory asymmetry depends in part on whether space is perceived as divided into left and right sides, and in part of the balance between spatial and verbal requirements. Both factors, and the asymmetry itself, may interact with sex.  相似文献   
58.
A taxonomy of part-whole or meronymic relations is developed to explain the ordinary English-speaker's use of the term “part of” and its cognates. The resulting classification yields six types of meronymic relations: 1. component-integral object (pedal-bike), 2. member-collection (ship-fleet), 3. portion-mass (slice-pie), 4. stuff-object (steel-car), 5. feature-activity (paying-shopping), and 6. place-area (Everglades-Florida). Meronymic relations ore further distinguished from other inclusion relations, such as spatial inclusion, and class inclusion, and from several other semantic relations: attribution, attachment, and ownership. This taxonomy is then used to explain cases of apparent intransitivity in merological syllogisms, and standard form syllogisms whose premises express different inclusion relations. The data suggest that intransitivities arise due to equivocations between different types of semantic relations. These results are then explained by means of the relation element theory which accounts for the character and behavior of semantic relations in terms of more primitive relational elements. The inferential phenomena observed are then explained by means of a single principle of element matching.  相似文献   
59.
We carried out a randomized controlled trial to determine whether an intensive intervention after a suicide attempt could decrease by half the risk of a repeat attempt in the following two years. After initial assessment and randomization, experimental subjects attended 18 therapy appointments over one year, including one home visit, and measures to improve attendance. Control subjects received the usual care. Of 63 experimental subjects, 35% made a repeat attempt, and so did 30% of 63 control subjects. The study had a 99% power to detect the desired decrease of risk (30% to 15%). Clearly, the intervention did not achieve its objective.  相似文献   
60.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号