首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   53706篇
  免费   2135篇
  国内免费   30篇
  2020年   606篇
  2019年   760篇
  2018年   1101篇
  2017年   1097篇
  2016年   1145篇
  2015年   773篇
  2014年   953篇
  2013年   4676篇
  2012年   1706篇
  2011年   1863篇
  2010年   1082篇
  2009年   1102篇
  2008年   1683篇
  2007年   1664篇
  2006年   1477篇
  2005年   1330篇
  2004年   1303篇
  2003年   1189篇
  2002年   1228篇
  2001年   1775篇
  2000年   1676篇
  1999年   1299篇
  1998年   691篇
  1997年   598篇
  1996年   548篇
  1995年   539篇
  1994年   523篇
  1993年   542篇
  1992年   1025篇
  1991年   970篇
  1990年   963篇
  1989年   871篇
  1988年   832篇
  1987年   829篇
  1986年   824篇
  1985年   875篇
  1984年   690篇
  1983年   599篇
  1982年   507篇
  1979年   684篇
  1978年   503篇
  1976年   470篇
  1975年   610篇
  1974年   632篇
  1973年   606篇
  1972年   543篇
  1971年   480篇
  1968年   542篇
  1967年   537篇
  1966年   474篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
In three experiments we studied the relationship between contextual conditioning and the reinstatement of extinguished lever pressing that occurs when noncontingent food is introduced following extinction. In all three experiments the non-contingent food was presented off-baseline (with the response levers not present). On subsequent tests, with the response levers present, animals that had been exposed to food showed more reinstatement of lever pressing than control animals. This finding rules out alternative mechanisms for the reinstated responding that rely on the interaction of non-contingent food and responding, such as superstitious reinforcement or the discriminative after-effects of food. In addition, in each experiment we demonstrated that manipulations known to affect contextual conditioning (signalling the food in Experiment 1, context extinction in Experiment 2, and switching contexts in Experiment 3) reduced the reinstatement. These results are consistent with the claim that contextual conditioning is important in controlling instrumental conditioning and closely parallel findings concerning the reinstatement of Pavlovian responsing following extinction.  相似文献   
32.
33.
34.
35.
36.
37.
38.
By roughly 6 years of age, children acquire the stereotype that men are more competent than women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM), potentially leading to greater trust in scientific information provided by men. This study tested whether 3- to 8-year-old children differentially endorsed conflicting information about science and toys presented by male and female informants depicted as a ‘man’ and ‘woman’ (Exp1) or ‘scientists’ (Exp2). Children were expected to endorse toy testimony from gender-matched informants; thus, the key question concerned endorsement of science testimony. In Exp1 (N = 149), boys and girls showed a same-gender informant preference for toy testimony; however, girls endorsed the male informant's testimony more for science than for toys – but only when tested by a male experimenter. In Exp2 (N = 264), boys and girls showed a same-gender preference, irrespective of content. Findings suggest that STEM-related gender stereotypes might lead girls to trust scientific information presented by men over women in certain contexts.  相似文献   
39.
陈莲笙道长生于1917年10月25日,上海市人.历任中国道教协会副会长,中国道教协会顾问,上海市道教协会会长.上海市道教协会名誉会长,上海城隍庙住持,上海道学院院长,<上海道教>杂志主编,上海市宗教学会理事,上海市政协第七届委员,上海市政协第八、九届常委等职.  相似文献   
40.
Previous work on children's intuitive knowledge about the natural world has documented their difficulty in acquiring an overarching concept of biological life that includes plants as well as humans and non‐human animals. It has also suggested that the acquisition of fundamental biological concepts like alive and die may be influenced by the language used to describe them, as evidenced by differences between English‐ and Indonesian‐speaking children's performance in tasks involving these concepts. Here, we examine one particularly important source of linguistic information available to children during this acquisition process: everyday conversations with their parents. We take a cross‐linguistic approach in analysing the evidence available to English‐ and Indonesian‐speaking children as they acquire meanings for words corresponding to the concepts alive and die . Our analysis illustrates that young children acquiring English and Indonesian are faced with distinct problems, but that parental input in both languages does little to support the acquisition of broad, inclusive biological concepts.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号