首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   546篇
  免费   12篇
  558篇
  2019年   9篇
  2018年   9篇
  2017年   14篇
  2016年   15篇
  2015年   4篇
  2014年   14篇
  2013年   56篇
  2012年   19篇
  2011年   17篇
  2010年   14篇
  2009年   14篇
  2008年   19篇
  2007年   16篇
  2006年   15篇
  2005年   13篇
  2004年   7篇
  2003年   12篇
  2002年   20篇
  2001年   6篇
  2000年   10篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   14篇
  1997年   10篇
  1996年   4篇
  1995年   5篇
  1994年   6篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   8篇
  1991年   7篇
  1990年   8篇
  1988年   9篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   7篇
  1985年   6篇
  1984年   9篇
  1983年   10篇
  1982年   12篇
  1981年   15篇
  1980年   6篇
  1979年   7篇
  1978年   8篇
  1977年   4篇
  1976年   7篇
  1974年   7篇
  1973年   7篇
  1972年   4篇
  1971年   4篇
  1968年   6篇
  1967年   4篇
  1966年   4篇
排序方式: 共有558条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
61.
62.
63.
CONTEXT: Evaluating the efficacy of pediatric weight loss treatments is critical. OBJECTIVE: This is the first meta-analysis of the efficacy of RCTs comparing pediatric lifestyle interventions to no-treatment or information/education-only controls. DATA SOURCES: Medline, PsycINFO, and Cochrane Controlled Trials Register. STUDY SELECTION: Fourteen RCTs targetting change in weight status were eligible, yielding 19 effect sizes. DATA EXTRACTION: Standardized coding was used to extract information on design, participant characteristics, interventions, and results. DATA SYNTHESIS: For trials with no-treatment controls, the mean effect size was 0.75 (k = 9, 95% confidence interval [CI] = 0.52-0.98) at end of treatment and 0.60 (k = 4, CI = 0.27-0.94) at follow-up. For trials with information/education-only controls, the mean ES was 0.48 (k = 4, CI = 0.13-0.82) at end of treatment and 0.91 (k = 2, CI = 0.32-1.50) at follow-up. No moderator effects were identified. CONCLUSIONS: Lifestyle interventions for pediatric overweight are efficacious in the short term with some evidence for extended persistence. Future research is required to identify moderators and mediators and to determine the optimal length and intensity of treatment required to produce enduring changes in weight status.  相似文献   
64.
The conversations of 100 mother-child (mean age4.5 years) dyads about the children's school experienceswere examined for their emotional content. Dyads variedalong the dimensions of gender of child (53 girls; 57 boys), ethnicity (31African-American, 39 Anglo-American, and 40Mexican-American), and SES (55 lower and 55 higher).When compared to mother-son dyads, mother-daughter dyadsmade more emotional references, particularly when discussing topicsrelated to interpersonal relationships and whendiscussing emotions experienced by the daughters.Higher-SES dyads made more emotional references than did lower-SES dyads. While there was no main effectof ethnicity associated with the overall tendency todiscuss emotions, relative to other dyads,African-American dyads made more emotional referenceswhen discussing noninterpersonal, nonacademic topicsthan did other dyads, Anglo-American dyads made moreemotional references during the discussing of learningtopics, and Mexican-American dyads discussed emotion more in relation to interpersonal topics thandid African-American dyads.  相似文献   
65.
66.
Evidence is given to indicate that Lawley's formulas for the standard errors of maximum likelihood loading estimates do not produce exact asymptotic results. A small modification is derived which appears to eliminate this difficulty.The authors are indebted to Walter Kristof and Thomas Stroud for their helpful reviews of an earlier version of this paper and particularly to D. N. Lawley for his review, comments, and encouragement.  相似文献   
67.
The relative efficacy of the major techniques typically used in behavioral treatment programs for weight reduction was investigated using obese adult volunteers. Study 1 compared the effects of self-monitoring, self-control procedures, monetary rewards, aversive imagery and relaxation training. These procedures resulted in significantly greater weight reduction than either a no treatment group or subjects who graphed and recorded daily weight. Self-monitoring of daily caloric intake was as effective as the other methods, both singly and combined, over a 4 week treatment period. Study 2 compared the long-term effects of self-monitoring vs the full complement of behavioral techniques used in Study 1. The full behavior management program was significantly more effective, both during the treatment period and at 3 and 12 week follow-ups, although self-monitoring again produced substantial weight loss.  相似文献   
68.
69.
Pausing phenomena: Influence on the quality of instruction   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
This report summarizes work of 7 years on the influence of a variable called “wait-time” on the development of language and logic of children taking part in elementary science programs. Analysis of over 300 classroom tape recordings showed mean pauses to be on the order of 1 sec; that is, after a teacher asked a question, students had to begin a response within an average time of 1 sec. If they did not, the teacher typically repeated, rephrased or asked a different question, or called on others. A second wait-time is involved: when a student makes a response, the teacher normally reacts or asks another question within an average time of 0.9 sec. This study investigates the consequences of manipulating both species of wait-time. When mean wait-time of 3–5 sec is achieved through training, analysis of more than 900 tapes shows changed values on eight student variables: (1) the length of response (number of words) increases, (2) the number of unsolicited but appropriate responses increases, (3) failures to respond decrease, (4) incidence of speculative responses increases, (5) incidence of student-student comparisons of data increases, (6) incidence of evidence-inference statements increases, (7) frequency of student-initiated questions increases, and (8) the variety in type of verbal moves made by students increases. Servochart plots of recordings show that students discussing science phenomena tend to speak in bursts, with intervals of as much as 3–5 sec between bursts being fairly common provided they are not interrupted. The average post-student-response wait-time of 0.9 sec apparently intervenes between bursts to prevent completion of a thought. The classroom is conceptualized as a two-player game in which the quality of inquiry will tend to improve when there is a better equity in the distribution of moves between the players. The teacher is treated as one player and the collection of students as the second player. Changes in wait-time shift the game toward a more equitable state. Over time, a classroom or instructional group on the prolonged wait-time schedule undergoes certain spontaneous changes. Two teacher variables change: (1) response flexibility scores increase, and (2) teacher questioning patterns becomes more variable. There is some indication that teacher expectations for performance of students rated as relatively slow improves.  相似文献   
70.
This article reports a second-order factor analysis of the 13 interpretable first-order factors of Adkins and Lyerly based on 66 variables for 200 Army men. The approach to simple structure for 6 second-order factors was unusually good. Loadings of the original 66 variables on the second-order factors were also obtained. Five of the factors are tentatively interpreted as Precision in Formation and Use of Verbal Concepts, General Verbal Fluency, Visualizing Spatial Constancy During Movement, Speed in Analysis, and Flexibility in Analysis.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号