首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   937篇
  免费   29篇
  2020年   14篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   19篇
  2017年   24篇
  2016年   29篇
  2015年   14篇
  2014年   27篇
  2013年   120篇
  2012年   37篇
  2011年   40篇
  2010年   20篇
  2009年   21篇
  2008年   22篇
  2007年   36篇
  2006年   27篇
  2005年   18篇
  2004年   20篇
  2003年   19篇
  2002年   26篇
  2001年   12篇
  2000年   18篇
  1999年   14篇
  1998年   19篇
  1997年   12篇
  1994年   9篇
  1993年   11篇
  1992年   13篇
  1991年   8篇
  1990年   18篇
  1989年   7篇
  1988年   14篇
  1987年   18篇
  1986年   8篇
  1985年   13篇
  1984年   13篇
  1983年   14篇
  1982年   12篇
  1981年   15篇
  1980年   9篇
  1979年   12篇
  1978年   10篇
  1977年   7篇
  1976年   7篇
  1975年   8篇
  1974年   10篇
  1973年   14篇
  1971年   8篇
  1969年   7篇
  1968年   11篇
  1966年   8篇
排序方式: 共有966条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
881.
We employed aspects of both attachment theory and social learning theory to develop an understanding of sex differences in young adults’ attachment to God, their concepts of God, and other aspects of their religiosity. We found that attachment to God is more likely to be associated with attachment to the same-sex parent than to the opposite-sex parent. We found that this is also true for concepts of God as loving, controlling, and distant in female, but not male participants in our study. For both males and females, mother’s level of religious involvement when they were growing up was associated with participants’ later attendance at religious services, but for males, attachment to father was an additional influence. In neither males nor females was attachment to either parent or their parent’s past religious involvement a significant predictor of strength of religious faith.  相似文献   
882.
Historians of philosophy are increasingly likely to emphasize the extent to which their work offers a pay‐off for philosophers of un‐historical or anti‐historical inclinations; but this defence is less familiar, and often seems less than self‐evident, to intellectual historians. This article examines this tendency, arguing that such arguments for the instrumental value of historical scholarship in philosophy are often more problematic than they at first appear. Using the relatively familiar case study of René Descartes' reading of his scholastic and Aristotelian contemporaries, the article attempts to problematize this notion of pay‐off from an historian's perspective.  相似文献   
883.
In this article, the relationship between Christian religiosity and the principles of social justice is explored, including the sociopolitical aspects of faith and advocacy. A particular emphasis is placed on the historical legacy and theological relationships between Christianity and social justice. The author concludes with a call for increasingly overt and intentional social justice work among counselors who self‐identify as Christian.  相似文献   
884.
We examined the Drive for Leanness Scale's gender invariance and its relationships with health-related behavior and body image-related drives. Men (N = 342) and women (N = 309) attending British universities completed the Drive for Leanness Scale, Drive for Thinness Scale, Drive for Muscularity Scale, and a demographic inventory. Support for configural and metric, but not scalar, invariance emerged. Drive for leanness was positively correlated with weight training frequency, supplement use, drive for thinness, and drive for muscularity in both genders. Results provide guidance on comparing Drive for Leanness Scale scores across gender and contribute to a coherent understanding of the drive for leanness and its correlates.  相似文献   
885.
G W Stuart  M Edwards  M L Cook 《Perception》1992,21(6):717-729
Recently it has been claimed by Livingstone and Hubel that, of three anatomically and functionally distinct visual channels (the magnocellular, parvocellular interblob, and blob channels), only the magnocellular channel is involved in the processing of stereoscopic depth. Since the magnocellular system shows little overt colour opponency, the reported loss of the ability to resolve random-dot stereograms defined only by colour contrast seems consistent with this view. However, Julesz observed that reversed-contrast stereograms could be fused if correlated colour information was added. In the present study, 'noise' (non-corresponding) pixels were injected into random-dot stereograms in order to increase fusion time. All six subjects tested were able to achieve stereopsis in less than three minutes when there was only correspondence in colour and not in luminance, and three when luminance contrast was completely reversed. This ability depends on information about the direction of colour contrast, not just the presence of chromatic borders. When luminance and chromatic contrast are defined in terms of signal-to-noise ratios at the photoreceptor mosaic, chromatic information plays at least as important a role in stereopsis as does luminance information, suggesting that the magnocellular channel is not uniquely involved.  相似文献   
886.
887.
888.
889.
890.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号