全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1860篇 |
免费 | 73篇 |
专业分类
1933篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 16篇 |
2020年 | 26篇 |
2019年 | 33篇 |
2018年 | 52篇 |
2017年 | 42篇 |
2016年 | 63篇 |
2015年 | 44篇 |
2014年 | 43篇 |
2013年 | 188篇 |
2012年 | 65篇 |
2011年 | 78篇 |
2010年 | 42篇 |
2009年 | 36篇 |
2008年 | 42篇 |
2007年 | 65篇 |
2006年 | 58篇 |
2005年 | 65篇 |
2004年 | 63篇 |
2003年 | 72篇 |
2002年 | 52篇 |
2001年 | 26篇 |
2000年 | 46篇 |
1999年 | 35篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 29篇 |
1996年 | 26篇 |
1995年 | 28篇 |
1994年 | 23篇 |
1993年 | 17篇 |
1992年 | 31篇 |
1991年 | 16篇 |
1990年 | 24篇 |
1989年 | 22篇 |
1988年 | 27篇 |
1987年 | 30篇 |
1986年 | 22篇 |
1985年 | 22篇 |
1984年 | 24篇 |
1983年 | 14篇 |
1982年 | 17篇 |
1981年 | 20篇 |
1980年 | 24篇 |
1978年 | 24篇 |
1977年 | 20篇 |
1976年 | 17篇 |
1975年 | 24篇 |
1974年 | 25篇 |
1973年 | 21篇 |
1972年 | 15篇 |
1966年 | 16篇 |
排序方式: 共有1933条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
102.
103.
Pinninti N Steer RA Rissmiller DJ Nelson S Beck AT 《Behaviour research and therapy》2002,40(9):1071-1079
To ascertain how useful the Beck Scale for Suicide Ideation (BSI; Beck & Steer, Manual for Beck Scale for Suicide Ideation (1991)) would be for assessing the severity of suicidal ideation in patients who were diagnosed with schizophrenia, schizoaffective, or bipolar disorders, 142 inpatients were asked to complete the BSI. Eight (6%) patients refused, and four patients (3%) were unable to complete the BSI because they were unable to concentrate. Of the 130 patients who completed the BSI, 53 (41%) had schizoaffective, 37 (28%) had paranoid schizophrenia, 30 (23%) had manic bipolar, and 10 (8%) had depressed bipolar disorders. The coefficient alpha for the BSI was .96, and its one-week test-retest reliability for a subsample of 15 inpatients was 0.88, p < 0.001. The BSI total scores were positively correlated with having ever attempted suicide, r = 0.46, p < 0.001. According to the BSI, 36 (28%) patients were classified as current suicide ideators. The results were discussed as supporting the use of the BSI with inpatients who are diagnosed with schizophrenia, schizoaffective, or bipolar disorders. 相似文献
104.
Frick-Horbury D 《The Journal of general psychology》2002,129(2):137-147
The author examined the effects of cueing for verbal recall with the accompanying self-generated hand gestures as a function of verbal skill. There were 36 participants, half with low SAT verbal scores and half with high SAT verbal scores. Half of the participants of each verbal-skill level were cued for recall with their own gestures, and the remaining half was given a free-recall test. Cueing with self-generated gestures aided the low-verbal-skill participants so that their retrieval rate equaled that of the high-verbal-skill participants and their loss of recall over a 2-week period was minimal. This effect was stable for both concrete and abstract words. The findings support the hypothesis that gestures serve as an auxiliary code for memory retrieval. 相似文献
105.
106.
Weiler EM Sandman DE Janson-Pinto J Dange AJ Dember WN Warm JS Nelson WT 《The Journal of general psychology》2000,127(4):365-371
Could monaural loudness adaptation be a simple artifact of psychophysical contrast? From adaptation data based on the Ipsilateral Comparison Paradigm (ICP), A. J. Dange, J. S. Warm, E. M. Weiler, and W. N. Dember (1993) concluded that loudness adaptation was not an artifact of psychophysical contrast, but their conclusion was dependent on results from one intensity. This study, involving multiple intensities, re-examined the issue of contrast versus adaptation and generally supported the conclusions of Dange et al. The results also showed an unexpected asymmetry of adaptation based on the direction of the referent modulation used with the ICP technique. Some implications are discussed. 相似文献
107.
Nelson JL 《Kennedy Institute of Ethics journal》2000,10(3):213-228
A fascinating criticism of abortion occasioned by prenatal diagnosis of potentially disabling traits is that the complex test-and-abortion sends a morally disparaging message to people living with disabilities. I have argued that available versions of this "expressivist" argument are inadequate on two grounds. The most fundamental is that, considered as a practice, abortions prompted by prenatal testing are not semantically well-behaved enough to send any particular message; they do not function as signs in a rule-governed symbol system. Further, even granting, for the sake of argument, the expressive power of testing and aborting, it would not be possible, contra the argument's proponents, to distinguish between abortions undertaken because of beliefs about the disabling conditions the fetus might face as a child and abortions undertaken for many other possible reasons -- e.g., because of the poverty the fetus would face or the increase in family size that the birth of a new child would occasion. Here, I respond to criticisms of those arguments, and propose and defend another: the expressivist argument cannot, in general, distinguish successfully between abortion and therapy as modalities for responding to disabilities. 相似文献
108.
Cued recall success varies with what people know and with what they do during an episode. This paper focuses on prior knowledge and disentangles the relative effects of 10 features of words and their relationships on cued recall. Results are reported for correlational and multiple regression analyses of data obtained from free association norms and from 29 experiments. The 10 features were only weakly correlated with each other in the norms and, with notable exceptions, in the experiments. The regression analysis indicated that forward cue-to-target strength explained the most variance, followed by backward target-to-cue strength. Target connectivity and set size explained the next most variance, along with mediated cue-to-target strength. Finally, frequency, concreteness, shared associate strength, and cue set size also contributed significantly to recall. Taken together, indices of prior word knowledge explain 49% of the recall variance. Theoretically driven equations that use free association to predict cued recall were also evaluated. Each equation was designed to condense multiple indices of word interconnectivity into a single predictor. 相似文献
109.
Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
Three experiments addressed factors that might influence whether or not young children take into account function, as opposed to overall appearance or shape, when they extend the names of novel artifacts. Experiment 1 showed that 4-year-olds more often extend a name on the basis of a demonstrated function when that function provides a plausible causal account of perceptible object structure. Experiment 2 showed that they more often extend a name by function when they respond slowly, and hence thoughtfully. Experiment 3 demonstrated that they are more likely to take function into account when they extend names than when they judge similarities. Comparisons of lexical and non-lexical conditions in younger children failed to show any differences. Overall, the findings suggest that by 4 years of age, children may learn names as labels for novel artifact kinds rather than perceptual classes, and that the processes by which they categorize may be mindful and reflective, as in adults. 相似文献
110.
Men have previously been reported to exhibit seasonal fluctuations on specific types of cognitive performance. It has been speculated that this performance variability is a result of changes in cerebral asymmetry due to lowered testosterone concentrations in the spring relative to the fall. In the present study, functional cerebral lateralization was measured in a group of men and women in the spring and fall. Free testosterone concentrations were assessed for participants to determine what associations might exist between seasonal variability in lateralization and seasonal fluctuations in testosterone exposure. Men and women tested in the spring exhibited exaggerated patterns of asymmetry compared to participants tested in the fall. Lower testosterone concentrations were observed in the spring compared to the fall in women, but not men. No direct associations between testosterone and lateralization were detected for either sex at either season. These results illustrate that seasonal fluctuations in testosterone exposure do not directly influence seasonal changes in functional lateralization patterns. 相似文献