全文获取类型
收费全文 | 583篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
586篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 18篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 25篇 |
2015年 | 17篇 |
2014年 | 19篇 |
2013年 | 71篇 |
2012年 | 20篇 |
2011年 | 23篇 |
2010年 | 20篇 |
2009年 | 13篇 |
2008年 | 11篇 |
2007年 | 24篇 |
2006年 | 25篇 |
2005年 | 24篇 |
2004年 | 26篇 |
2003年 | 23篇 |
2002年 | 15篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 14篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有586条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
401.
Rebecca J. Seward Donna M. Bayliss Jeneva L. Ohan 《International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology》2018,18(2):179-188
Background/Objective: Social vulnerability refers to difficulties detecting potentially harmful interpersonal situations. Although it is an important predictor of psychosocial and interpersonal difficulties in clinical samples, research investigating this construct is scarce. We aimed to (a) develop a brief measure for assessing social vulnerability in typically developing children, the Children's Social Vulnerability Questionnaire (CSVQ) (b) examine the relationship between social vulnerability and psychosocial functioning, (c) explore age-related differences, and (d) explore levels of social vulnerability amongst children with clinical needs. Method: Data were gathered on two samples. Participants were parents (n = 790) of elementary school-aged children (3-12 years), and parents and teachers of a second sample (n = 96). Results: Results provide strong reliability and validity evidence. Social vulnerability showed moderate relationships with emotional and behavioural problems, and only a weak relationship with social skills. Parents perceived greater social vulnerability in younger than older children, and amongst children with clinical needs. Parents’ and teachers’ scores were correlated. Conclusions: Social vulnerability is not simply a lack of social skill; rather, it is a valuable construct for understanding psychosocial risk, especially for young and clinical samples of children. 相似文献
402.
We attempted to replicate an intervention program by Bornstein and Quevillon (1976), which had shown that the disruptive classroom behavior of Head Start children could be dramatically reduced through self-instructional training. Although the subject population and procedures were quite similar across studies, our self-instructional training did not produce socially significant, durable increases in either appropriate classroom behavior or changes in teacher ratings of the children's behavior. These results suggest that additional variables may have been responsible for Bornstein and Quevillon's success. 相似文献
403.
H. Robert Outten Michael T. Schmitt Donna M. Garcia Nyla R. Branscombe 《Psychologie appliquee》2009,58(1):146-170
Using concepts from social identity theory ( Tajfel & Turner, 1979 ) and Lazarus and Folkman's (1984) cognitive theory of stress and coping we tested the capacity for group identification to foster beliefs in one's ability to cope successfully and in turn predict psychological well-being. Black American participants appraised the availability of coping options that varied by level of identity (individual, intragroup, and intergroup) as well as function of coping (problem-focused and emotion-focused). Replicating prior work, participants who were higher in racial group identification reported more positive well-being. Appraisals of individual emotion-focused and intergroup problem-focused options mediated the relationship of group identification with both self-esteem and life satisfaction. Appraisals of intergroup emotion-focused options also partially mediated the relationship between group identification and life satisfaction. Findings suggest that the relationship between minority group identification and well-being may partly be due to its influence over a person's sense that they and their group can respond effectively to disadvantage. 相似文献
404.
Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
405.
Yu JJ Lucero-Liu AA Gamble WC Taylor AR Christensen DH Modry-Mandell KL 《The Journal of psychology》2008,142(2):169-192
In this investigation, the authors explored the impact of individuals' cultural values on their partners' relationship adjustment and perceptions of their parenting relationship. The authors examined Mexican cultural values of simpatía (i.e., harmonious interpersonal relationships) and respeto (i.e., respect for authority figures) using a sample of 50 Mexican-origin couples in southern Arizona. Congruent with their hypotheses, results supported the proposition that fathers' simpatía is positively associated with both relationship adjustment and the parenting relationship as reported by mothers, whereas fathers' respeto is negatively associated with both relationship adjustment and the parenting relationship as reported by mothers. However, the authors found little evidence of a contribution of mothers' cultural values to fathers' perceptions of either relationship adjustment or the parenting relationship. They interpret these findings to suggest that mothers' relationship adjustment and parenting relationship are more sensitive to and dependent on fathers' degree of traditional cultural values among Mexican-origin families. 相似文献
406.
407.
Age effects on dual-task costs were examined in healthy adults (Exp. 1) and in typically developing children (Exp. 2). In both experiments, individual differences in performance on the single-task components were titrated so that any age
differences in dual-task costs could not be attributed to differences in single-task performance. Dual-task costs were found,
but there were no age-related differences in these costs in older relative to younger adults, in 7-year-old relative to 9-year-old
children, or across all four age groups. The results from these experiments suggest that previously reported age differences
in dual-task costs, in both healthy ageing and child development, may be due to a failure to adequately equate single-task
difficulty. 相似文献
408.
Leandra Godoy Melissa Long Donna Marschall Stacy Hodgkinson Brooke Bokor Hope Rhodes Howard Crumpton Mark Weissman Lee Beers 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2017,24(3-4):245-258
Behavioral health integration within primary care has been evolving, but literature traditionally focuses on smaller scale efforts. We detail how behavioral health has been integrated across a large, urban pediatric hospital system’s six primary care clinics (serving over 35,000 children annually and insured predominately through Medicaid) and discuss strategies for success in sustaining and expanding efforts to achieve effective integration of behavioral health into primary care. In a time span of 3 years, the clinics have implemented routine, universal behavioral health screening at well child visits, participated in a 15-month behavioral health screening quality improvement learning collaborative, and integrated the work of psychologists and psychiatrists. Additional work remains to be done in improving family engagement, further expanding services, and ensuring sustainability. 相似文献
409.
410.
Judy D. Sheeshka Donna M. Woolcott Neil J. Mackinnon 《Journal of applied social psychology》1993,23(19):1547-1573
Our focus in this study was the observed gap between informed awareness and the intention to act. We used elements of social cognitive theory to develop a theoretical model of six psychosocial factors hypothesized to influence the intention to adopt healthy eating behaviors. Survey data from 490 white-collar employees were analyzed using a Linear Structural Relations (LISREL) program and a stacked model. The overall fit of the model to one half of the data was excellent, x2 (6, 237) = 3.79, p= 0.71, and the model was successfully replicated with the second half of the data x2 (28, 249) = 28.68, p= 0.43. Our model suggests that self-efficacy expectations (a) partially depend on the perceived likelihood of an outcome; (b) act as a conduit for the effects of media, disincentives, and outcome expectancy; and (c) have powerful direct influences on intentions. As expected, outcome expectancy adds little to the prediction of intentions. 相似文献