全文获取类型
收费全文 | 789篇 |
免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
794篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 13篇 |
2019年 | 9篇 |
2018年 | 22篇 |
2017年 | 20篇 |
2016年 | 31篇 |
2015年 | 21篇 |
2014年 | 26篇 |
2013年 | 94篇 |
2012年 | 26篇 |
2011年 | 29篇 |
2010年 | 25篇 |
2009年 | 19篇 |
2008年 | 20篇 |
2007年 | 28篇 |
2006年 | 30篇 |
2005年 | 28篇 |
2004年 | 29篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 22篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 15篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 15篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 10篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 10篇 |
1981年 | 10篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1967年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有794条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
201.
This study examined the effect of provocateurs' emotion displays on first through fourth graders' social information processing (SIP). Rating and nomination sociometric techniques were used to identify rejected-aggressive, rejected-nonaggressive, average-nonaggressive, and popular-nonaggressive groups. Children viewed videotaped ambiguous provocation situations in which provocateurs' emotion displays were varied systematically. After each vignette, children's recall of story details, attribution of intent, and problem-solving responses were obtained. Half of the children were randomly assigned to a condition where they were asked directly about provocateurs' emotions, and the remaining children were not asked about provocateurs' feelings. Results revealed both developmental and social adjustment differences in SIP. In addition, provocateurs' emotion displays and asking versus not asking about those displays influenced multiple aspects of SIP. 相似文献
202.
This research examined the association of having a tattoo and engaging in premarital sexual intercourse. Data gathered from a convenience sample of 450 college students indicated that tattooed respondents were substantively and significantly more likely to be sexually active than nontattooed college students. Tattooed men became sexually active at a significantly earlier age than nontattooed men but no such difference was found between tattooed and nontattooed college women. 相似文献
203.
OBJECTIVE: To develop a predictive model of the lift-off event during chair rise in healthy subjects, using foot-floor reaction forces. BACKGROUND: An important event during chair rise is lift-off from the seat: the transition from the inherently stable three-point contact to the unstable two-point contact. There is no consistent or generally agreed upon method for estimating the time of lift-off when an instrumented seat is unavailable. METHODS: Twenty healthy volunteers were divided into a testing set and training set. Each subject performed repeated chair rise trials at different speeds. Seat-floor and foot-floor forces, recorded with two force platforms, were used to develop a model of the lift-off event. RESULTS: The magnitude of the vertical foot-floor reaction at lift-off (F0VF) was linearly related (R2 = 0.71, P < 0.001) to the peak vertical foot-floor reaction force (FMVF). A linear model was developed for the training group, which enabled prediction of lift-off time for the testing group with an absolute average error of 6 ms (about 1 data frame at 150 Hz). The linear model derived for the entire sample was: F0VF = 28.14 + FMVF * (0.6434). CONCLUSIONS: The lift-off event for healthy subjects performing chair rise can be accurately predicted from foot-floor reaction forces, without requiring an instrumented seat. 相似文献
204.
Morrison I Lloyd D di Pellegrino G Roberts N 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2004,4(2):270-278
Results obtained with functional magnetic resonance imaging show that both feeling a moderately painful pinprick stimulus
to the fingertips and witnessing another person’s hand undergo similar stimulation are associated with common activity in
a pain-related area in the right dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Common activity in response to noxious tactile and
visual stimulation was restricted to the right inferior Brodmann’s area 24b. These results suggest a shared neural substrate
for felt and seen pain for aversive ecological events happening to strangers and in the absence of overt symbolic cues. In
contrast to ACC 24b, the primary somatosensory cortex showed significant activations in response to both noxious and innocuous
tactile, but not visual, stimuli. The different response patterns in the two areas are consistent with the ACC’s role in coding
the motivational-affective dimension of pain, which is associated with the preparation of behavioral responses to aversive
events. 相似文献
205.
This study examined the effects of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID) version on developmental outcomes among drug-exposed children, some of whom received an intervention. Developmental outcome was evaluated with the BSID at 12 and 18 months and with the BSID-II at 24 and 30 months. In the repeated measures analyses, children scored higher on the BSID Mental Developmental Index (MDI; p < .01) and Psychomotor Developmental Index (PDI; p < .01) than on the BSID-II MDI and PDI. Version x Time (p < .01) and Version x Group (p < .01) interactions were also found for the MDI but not the PDI. PDI scores decreased on both the BSID and the BSID-II (p < .01). Over the first 2 years postpartum, mean MDI and PDI scores decreased among these high-risk, drug-exposed children. 相似文献
206.
The complexities of complex span: explaining individual differences in working memory in children and adults 总被引:7,自引:0,他引:7
Bayliss DM Jarrold C Gunn DM Baddeley AD 《Journal of experimental psychology. General》2003,132(1):71-92
Two studies are presented that investigated the constraints underlying working memory performance in children and adults. In each case, independent measures of processing efficiency and storage capacity are assessed to determine their relative importance in predicting performance on complex span tasks,which measure working memory capacity. Results show that complex span performance was independently constrained by individual differences in domain-general processing efficiency and domain-specific storage capacity. Residual variance, which may reflect the ability to coordinate storage and processing, also predicted academic achievement. These results challenge the view that complex span taps a limited-capacity resource pool shared between processing and storage operations. Rather, they are consistent with a multiple-component model in which separate resource pools support the processing and storage functions of working memory. 相似文献
207.
David Fleming 《Argumentation》1998,12(2):147-166
In this paper, I explore connections between two disciplines not typically linked: argumentation theory and urban design. I first trace historical ties between the art of reasoned discourse and the idea of civic virtue. I next analyze discourse norms implicit in three theories of urban design: Jane Jacobs' The Death and Life of Great American Cities (1961), Christopher Alexander's A Pattern Language: Towns, Buildings, Construction (1977), and Peter Katz's The New Urbanism: Toward an Architecture of Community (1994). I then propose a set of settlement issues of potential interest to both urban designers and argumentation theorists: size, density, heterogeneity, publicity, security, and identity. I conclude by suggesting that the good city be seen as both a spatial and a discursive entity. From such a perspective, good public discourse is dependent, at least in part, on good public space; and good public space is defined, at least in part, as a context conducive to good public discourse. 相似文献
208.
Patrick W. Corrigan E. Paul Holmes Daniel Luchins Joseph Parks Abdul Basit Ethel Delaney Donna Kayton-Weinberg 《Behavioral Interventions》1994,9(1):1-12
Despite the success that behavior therapy has demonstrated in treating severely mentally ill adults, widespread impact of behavioral treatments on this population has been limited because the staff of many inpatient settings do not routinely utilize these strategies. Surveying staff regarding their perception of programatic and organizational needs is proposed as a valuable first step for selecting behavioral strategies to be introduced in these settings. Goldfried and D'Zurilla (1969) developed a behavioral assessment survey that is especially useful for identifying staff needs vis-à-vis behavioral rehabilitation. Using these strategies, survey questions addressed five problem areas: Administrative, Staff, Patient, Resource, and Programatic. Results using this survey with 40 clinicians on the extended care unit of a state hospital showed that staff members had greatest concern with the Patient Problem Area (i.e., aversive patient behaviors that are not sufficiently addressed by treatment plans). Further analyses showed staff members were interested in addressing Patient concerns using incentive procedures. The needs assessment in this study not only provided useful information that might be generalized to other treatment settings, but also showcased a reliable survey approach that program developers might implement prior to designing training curricula for behaviorally naive staff in inpatient settings. 相似文献
209.
Joseph S. Lalli Elizabeth Pinter-Lalli F. Charles Mace Donna M. Murphy 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1991,24(1):167-174
This study was a replication and extension of research by Foxx, McMorrow, Bittle, and Ness (1986) that assessed generalization effects of a social skills training program on the interactional behavior of adults with developmental disabilities. Target skills were a verbal action or reaction in six skill areas that specifically addressed the participants' skill deficits. In the present study, we trained 5 adult residents of a group home across these six skill areas using the “Sorry” game format and the scoring criteria described by Foxx et al. We extended the results of Foxx et al. by (a) using pretreatment assessment procedures to identify participants' specific skill deficits, (b) training all residents in the natural environment, (c) training participant—participant interactions, (d) training participants to respond to four of the six skill areas through the use of a role-play procedure, and (e) omitting rewards, criterion levels, and self-monitoring. Additionally, the trainer in the present study modeled correct responses only as an error correction procedure during training. Similar to those of Foxx et al., our results indicated that all participants increased their use of the trained interactional behaviors during the generalization assessments in the presence of other trained peers. 相似文献
210.