首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2265篇
  免费   76篇
  2018年   22篇
  2017年   21篇
  2016年   36篇
  2015年   21篇
  2014年   26篇
  2013年   230篇
  2012年   57篇
  2011年   58篇
  2010年   41篇
  2009年   59篇
  2008年   55篇
  2007年   68篇
  2006年   53篇
  2005年   50篇
  2004年   68篇
  2003年   65篇
  2002年   71篇
  2001年   32篇
  2000年   36篇
  1999年   29篇
  1998年   36篇
  1997年   35篇
  1996年   28篇
  1995年   36篇
  1994年   36篇
  1993年   26篇
  1992年   37篇
  1991年   26篇
  1990年   31篇
  1989年   26篇
  1988年   27篇
  1987年   24篇
  1986年   31篇
  1985年   29篇
  1984年   40篇
  1983年   39篇
  1982年   35篇
  1981年   52篇
  1980年   43篇
  1979年   48篇
  1978年   38篇
  1977年   36篇
  1976年   47篇
  1975年   51篇
  1974年   50篇
  1973年   41篇
  1972年   25篇
  1971年   29篇
  1969年   33篇
  1968年   31篇
排序方式: 共有2341条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
941.
942.
Levels, hierarchies, and the locus of control   总被引:7,自引:0,他引:7  
The Bartlett--Craik view of human performance is restated; and particularly that it is organised at different levels. On that view, the lower levels are controlled by the upper but capable of functioning independently. Modern views of memory, language, and problem-solving are compared with this doctrine, and found to embody some of its virtues but not all. Fresh experiments are described, in which people take decisions about the running of a transportation system. The simplest control mechanism which will model their behaviour is a two-level adaptive controller.  相似文献   
943.
Ratings of orthographic distinctiveness were obtained for 139 homonym pairs. Mean ratings on a 9-point scale ranged from 7.75 to 2.44. Reliability of the ratings was high (r = .91). In addition, orthographic distinctiveness was found to be independent of disparity in perceived meanings of the separate homonym forms.  相似文献   
944.
A one-trial-a-day discrete-trial avoidance conditioning procedure run for 30 consecutive days was compared with a massed-trial procedure where Ss received 30 consecutive trials within 1 day. The Ss in both groups were equated for time each S spent in the nonshock compartment of the apparatus, number of times the transport box carrying Ss was lifted out of the nonshock compartment, and the number of times each S was handled. The main difference between groups was the intersession trial length of 24 h for the one-trial-a-day Ss. Learning was rapid for both groups. The groups did not differ reliably on six acquisition indices. The methodological advantages of the one-trial-a-day procedure and its theoretical importance were discussed.  相似文献   
945.
Perceptual masking was studied under binocular and dichoptic conditions in order to separate peripheral and central interference effects. Under binocular conditions, when the test flash (TF) and the blanking flash (BF) fell on both retinas, both retroactive and proactive masking were demonstrated. Under dichoptic conditions, when the TF fell on one eye and the BF on the other eye, thus eliminating opportunity for intraretinal interference, there was partial retroactive perceptual masking and no proactive masking. These results suggest that binocular proactive masking is due to peripheral light adaptation, that binocular retroactive masking is due to both peripheral and central effects, and that dichoptic masking is due solely to central retrochiasmal interference. It is proposed that dichoptic retroactive perceptual masking affords a method of investigating central perception time, i.e., the time to consolidate a perceptual experience.  相似文献   
946.
Luminance thresholds for downward moving contours were measured under several conditions of adaptation. Included was one condition which desensitized visual mechanisms responsive to downward motion. Another condition exerted equal effects on both up- and down-sensitive mechanisms. Thresholds for moving contours were unaffected by exposure to contours which moved in the opposite direction. This indicates that the perception of motion does not depend upon the relative activity in oppositely tuned, directionally selective visual mechanisms.  相似文献   
947.
Rhesus monkeys were trained to make auditory frequency, intensity, and duration discriminations. Initial training was carried out with a procedure which allowed the utilization of either relational cues or absolute cues. Later tests with similar auditory parameters allowed only the utilization of relational cues. Performance on these latter tests indicated that relational rather than absolute cues had been utilized when both types were available on the frequency and intensity discrimination tasks. However, absolute rather than relational cues were apparently utilized in making duration discriminations.  相似文献   
948.
Taste profiles were obtained for 26 compounds after adaptation to distilled water and also for water after adaptation to each of the 26 compounds. Each of the four “basic tastes” was induced in water by adaptation to certain of the compounds. Compounds having similar tastes did not necessarily have similar water tastes. The results imply a peripheral locus of the water taste mechanism(s).  相似文献   
949.
Patterns were computer-generated about two prototypes to form schematic clusters of three diameters about the cluster centroids. In a schematic concept-formation task, recovery of a priori schematic class membership by Os varied inversely as physical cluster diameter, with concept acquisition across trials evidenced in only the low-cluster-diameter condition. For each 0 who failed to classify according to the schema rule, linear discriminant function analysis was applied to his classes. In all cases, O-generated classes were successfully recovered by the physical pattern features used as predictors, the mapping of these classes by the LDF exceeding that by the schema rule in accuracy at all cluster-diameter levels.  相似文献   
950.
Users of interobserver agreement statistics have heretofore ignored the problem of autocorrelation in behavior sequences when testing the statistical significance of agreement measures. Due to autocorrelation traditional reliability tests based on the 2 × 2 contingency-table model (e.g., kappa, phi) are incorrect. Correct tests can be developed by using the bivariate time series as a statistical model. Seen from this perspective, testing the significance of interobserver agreement becomes formally equivalent to testing the significance of the lag-zero cross-correlation between two time series. The robust procedure known as the jackknife is suggested for this purpose.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号