全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5548篇 |
免费 | 74篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
5623篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 55篇 |
2023年 | 61篇 |
2022年 | 87篇 |
2021年 | 104篇 |
2020年 | 142篇 |
2019年 | 136篇 |
2018年 | 265篇 |
2017年 | 249篇 |
2016年 | 228篇 |
2015年 | 147篇 |
2014年 | 166篇 |
2013年 | 703篇 |
2012年 | 401篇 |
2011年 | 408篇 |
2010年 | 253篇 |
2009年 | 166篇 |
2008年 | 319篇 |
2007年 | 258篇 |
2006年 | 227篇 |
2005年 | 210篇 |
2004年 | 121篇 |
2003年 | 129篇 |
2002年 | 125篇 |
2001年 | 58篇 |
2000年 | 76篇 |
1999年 | 45篇 |
1998年 | 25篇 |
1997年 | 18篇 |
1996年 | 21篇 |
1995年 | 18篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 16篇 |
1992年 | 30篇 |
1991年 | 21篇 |
1990年 | 24篇 |
1989年 | 23篇 |
1988年 | 26篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 12篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1979年 | 15篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1976年 | 13篇 |
1974年 | 10篇 |
1973年 | 10篇 |
1971年 | 10篇 |
1970年 | 10篇 |
1968年 | 11篇 |
1965年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有5623条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
Functional models of face recognition and speech production have developed separately. However, naming a familiar face is, of course, an act of speech production. In this paper we propose a revision of Bruce and Young's (1986) model of face processing, which incorporates two features of Levelt's (1989) model of speech production. In particular, the proposed model includes two stages of lexical access for names and monitoring of face naming based on a "perceptual loop". Two predictions were derived from the perceptual loop hypothesis of speech monitoring: (1) naming errors in which a (correct) rare surname is erroneously replaced by a common surname should occur more frequently than the reverse substitution (the error asymmetry effect); (2) naming errors in which a common surname is articulated are more likely to be repaired than errors which result in articulation of a rare surname (the error-repairing effect). Both predictions were supported by an analysis of face naming errors in a laboratory face naming task. In a further experiment we considered the possibility that the effects of surname frequency observed in face naming errors could be explained by the frequency sensitivity of lexical access in speech production. However, no effect of the frequency of the surname of the faces used in the previous experiment was found on face naming latencies. Therefore, it is concluded that the perceptual loop hypothesis provides the more parsimonious account of the entire pattern of the results. 相似文献
3.
Abibliography on subjective contours and a brief summary of trends in research-on-thin problem are presented. The bibliography covers the years 1900–1990 and contains 445 entries, each briefly annotated with a code that indicates the general content and theoretical orientation of the item. 相似文献
4.
5.
A general structural equation model with dichotomous,ordered categorical,and continuous latent variable indicators 总被引:21,自引:0,他引:21
Bengt Muthén 《Psychometrika》1984,49(1):115-132
A structural equation model is proposed with a generalized measurement part, allowing for dichotomous and ordered categorical variables (indicators) in addition to continuous ones. A computationally feasible three-stage estimator is proposed for any combination of observed variable types. This approach provides large-sample chi-square tests of fit and standard errors of estimates for situations not previously covered. Two multiple-indicator modeling examples are given. One is a simultaneous analysis of two groups with a structural equation model underlying skewed Likert variables. The second is a longitudinal model with a structural model for multivariate probit regressions.This research was supported by Grant No. 81-IJ-CX-0015 from the National Institute of Justice, by Grant No. DA 01070 from the U.S. Public Health Service, and by Grant No. SES-8312583 from the National Science Foundation. I thank Julie Honig for drawing the figures. Requests for reprints should be sent to Bengt Muthén, Graduate School of Education, University of California, Los Angeles, California 90024. 相似文献
6.
To illustrate the view that a speaker can have a partial understanding of a concept, Burge uses the example of Leibniz’s and Newton’s understanding of the concept of derivative. In a recent article, Sheldon Smith criticizes this example and maintains that Newton’s and Leibniz’s use of their derivative symbols does not univocally determine their references. The present article aims at challenging Smith’s analysis. It first shows that Smith misconstrues Burge’s position. It second suggests that the philosophical lessons one should draw from the practice of the historians of philosophy are more ambivalent than what Smith thinks.
相似文献7.
8.
M. B. Sterman J. J. Lynch M. T. Orne D. Paskewitz J. Costello N. Nicolov O. Diankov M. Popova E. Tsvetanska Robert G. Grenell J. J. Izquierdo V. H. Mark Joseph Germana N. Zill William P. Paré Joseph E. O. Newton George C. Offutt Walter Ehrlich Joseph E. D. Newton Julij Tosef J. Perez-Cruet Chester R. Wilpizeski John F. Lontz Andrew Livingston Joseph W. Cullen Samuel A. Corson Herman R. Weed Elizabeth O. Corson O. D. Murphree Paul N. Morgan Ruth Jarman J. Antal T. A. Ban J. V. Ananth H. E. Lehmann A. Ulric Moore Richard H. Barnes Wilson G. Pond 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1970,5(3):185-193
9.
10.
Stig Fhanér 《Psychometrika》1966,31(2):267-269
It is shown that certain correlation measures suggested by information theory are simple functions of the determinants of the correlation matrices involved, if multivariate normality is assumed. This illuminates and in a sense strengthens Rozeboom's point in [3], that information theoretical statistics have classical correlational counterparts. 相似文献