全文获取类型
收费全文 | 794篇 |
免费 | 33篇 |
专业分类
827篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 9篇 |
2018年 | 15篇 |
2017年 | 15篇 |
2016年 | 28篇 |
2015年 | 38篇 |
2014年 | 37篇 |
2013年 | 96篇 |
2012年 | 49篇 |
2011年 | 54篇 |
2010年 | 39篇 |
2009年 | 42篇 |
2008年 | 47篇 |
2007年 | 46篇 |
2006年 | 27篇 |
2005年 | 19篇 |
2004年 | 27篇 |
2003年 | 19篇 |
2002年 | 17篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 16篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有827条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
A new chronometric procedure, the Implicit Association Procedure (IAP), was adapted to assess the implicit personality self-concept of shyness. A sample of 300 participants completed a shyness-inducing role play and, before or after the role play, a shyness IAP, a shyness Implicit Association Test (IAT), and direct self-ratings. The experimental group was instructed to fake nonshyness. The control group did not receive this instruction. IAT and IAP were unaffected by position effects, and were less susceptible to faking than direct self-ratings with regard to mean levels and correlates. Under faking, correlations between direct and indirect measures decreased, and direct but not indirect measures showed higher correlations with social desirability and lower correlations with observed shyness. Despite many similarities, the true correlation between IAT and IAP was estimated only .61, indicating high method-specific variance in both procedures. The findings suggest that indirect measures are more robust against faking than traditional self-ratings but do not yet meet psychometric criteria for practical assessment purposes. 相似文献
22.
Dipl.-Psych. Matthias Richter 《Forum der Psychoanalyse》2012,28(1):27-49
The somewhat crude question of whether we need neuropsychotherapy means more precisely whether its neuroscientific perspective can be useful for the psychotherapeutic practice. To answer this question the traditional scientific approaches do not completely suffice. The ??empirical-technical approach?? of quantitative psychotherapy research is of no service here as neuropsychotherapy is only its continuation. Within the framework of this methodology it is not possible to critically reflect on the relevance of neuroscientific research. Such a reflection is, however, the prerequisite for an evaluation of the benefits of neuropsychotherapy. The question about the meaning of neurobiological processes thus leads to the field of epistemology. This ??theoretical approach?? is nonetheless only helpful within limits. It provides an important background of reflection, however it needs to remain abstract compared to psychotherapeutic practice because it does not integrate the specific goals of such practice. What is presented here instead is an ??approach of pragmatic reflection??. This reflection departs from a general purpose of psychotherapy from which the value of neuropsychotherapy can be appropriately estimated. After this clarification the approach of pragmatic reflection can be preliminarily demonstrated with concrete applications of neuroscience in psychotherapy. What is shown hereby is that neuropsychotherapy represents a very specific practice of argumention and action, which implements a logic into psychotherapy that does not fit its contents and goals. 相似文献
23.
24.
Dipl.-Psych. Andrea Hirschmann 《Psychotherapeut》2007,52(3):218-222
25.
Rainer Dyckerhoff 《Journal of Multi-Criteria Decision Analysis》1994,3(1):41-58
In expected utility many results have been derived that give necessary and/or sufficient conditions for a multivariate utility function to be decomposable into lower-dimensional functions. In particular, multilinear, multiplicative and additive decompositions have been widely discussed. These utility functions can be more easily assessed in practical situations. In this paper we present a theory of decomposition in the context of nonadditive expected utility such as anticipated utility or Choquet expected utility. We show that many of the results used in conventional expected utility carry over to these more general frameworks. If preferences over lotteries depend only on the marginal probability distributions, then in expected utility the utility function is additively decomposable. We show that in anticipated utility the marginality condition implies not only that the utility function is additively decomposable but also that the distortion function is the identity function. We further demonstrate that a decision maker who is bivariate risk neutral has a utility function that is additively decomposable and a distortion function q for which q(½) = ½. 相似文献
26.
Madiha?Rana Majeed?RanaEmail author Philipp?Y.?Herzberg Christin?Krause 《Journal of religion and health》2015,54(6):2142-2154
Little research has been done on comparing confessions regarding mental health. In the present study, 320 people (78 Buddhists, 77 Catholics, 89 Protestants and 79 Muslims) were compared in terms of their symptom severity. Buddhists and Protestants had lower scores than Catholics and Muslims for obsessive–compulsive behavior and hostility. Muslim group had the highest comparative scores for psychoticism. Buddhists and Protestants had comparatively low scores for paranoid ideation and overall symptom severity, with Catholics and Muslims having high ones. Results reveal that confession should be taken in account in psychological research and diagnosis, since it is explicitly associated with psychological well-being. 相似文献
27.
28.
84 student teachers from undergraduate classes at a western university in Kansas completed a 9-question checklist of learning disabilities before interacting with a behavioral disordered/learning disabilities teacher, a paraprofessional, and a high school learning-disabled student. After the dialog, the student teachers completed a Social Readjustment Rating Scale, Breskin Rigidity Test, the Self-evaluation Questionnaire: State and Trait Anxiety, and the same 9-question checklist with one additional question ("Now that you have seen a learning-disabled student has it affected your opinion of learning disabilities?"). The dialog affected 63% of the students' opinions while 31% stated the presentation did not affect their opinion (6% were missing or not completed). Persons who experienced more life-changes and problems tended to rate or view learning disabilities more negatively. The more trait anxiety experienced by the students, the lower they rated learning disabilities after the presentation. 相似文献
29.
Rainer Scheuchenpflug 《Acta psychologica》1999,100(3):109-242
Current models of face representation involve the notion of a high-dimensional face space. Computational models of face space based on principal components analysis (PCA) have been successfully used to predict human judgements of face sex or race. In this work the capability of PCA-based face spaces to predict human judgements of face similarity is examined. Three different paradigms were used. In Experiment 1 subjects learned face-name associations for 18 faces and identified these faces on tachistoscopic presentation. The number of confusions was used as a measure of face similarity. In Experiment 2 the same subjects subjectively rated the similarity of all 153 possible face pairs. In Experiment 3 reaction time to identify a face in an odd-man-out task was measured as an index of face similarity. These empirical measures were correlated with distance of the faces in PCA-based spaces of different dimensionalities. For Experiments 1 and 2 these correlations were highest for one-or two-dimensional face spaces (r=−0.27 vs. −0.28). For Experiment 3 the correlation was highest for a space consisting of 13 dimensions (r=−0.51). Thus PCA-based spaces seem capable to predict human similarity judgements to some extent. Possible reasons for the differences in predictability between paradigms are discussed. 相似文献
30.