首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   634篇
  免费   17篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2023年   7篇
  2022年   6篇
  2021年   6篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   21篇
  2018年   17篇
  2017年   15篇
  2016年   26篇
  2015年   7篇
  2014年   15篇
  2013年   59篇
  2012年   42篇
  2011年   23篇
  2010年   22篇
  2009年   20篇
  2008年   20篇
  2007年   20篇
  2006年   18篇
  2005年   17篇
  2004年   12篇
  2003年   13篇
  2002年   10篇
  2001年   11篇
  2000年   8篇
  1999年   9篇
  1993年   8篇
  1991年   5篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   9篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   8篇
  1986年   5篇
  1985年   8篇
  1983年   5篇
  1981年   7篇
  1980年   7篇
  1979年   9篇
  1978年   5篇
  1977年   6篇
  1976年   7篇
  1975年   7篇
  1973年   8篇
  1972年   9篇
  1971年   7篇
  1970年   7篇
  1968年   9篇
  1967年   8篇
  1966年   6篇
  1964年   5篇
  1958年   5篇
排序方式: 共有653条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
581.
Pastoral Psychology -  相似文献   
582.
583.
584.
Since the late 20th century, public discourse in Muslim-minority countries has centered around the question of how to classify Muslims. In this paper, we compare the state, academic, and self-classification of Muslims in two countries: the United Kingdom and Germany. We propose that the historical experience of anti-Semitism makes religion a more salient master category to understand Muslims in Germany, while the history of both anti-Semitism and anti-Black racism largely resulting from colonial domination means that religion together with race are master categories used to understand Muslims in the United Kingdom. Through this multilayered ethnographic and historical analysis, we challenge taken-for-granted assumptions in both the political and academic milieu about what it means to be Muslim, emphasizing the importance of the interplay between sociopolitical categories and self-identifications.  相似文献   
585.
Porntida Tanjitpiyanond  Jolanda Jetten  Kim Peters  Ashwini Ashokkumar  Oumar Barry  Matthew Billet  Maja Becker  Robert W. Booth  Diego Castro  Juana Chinchilla  Giulio Costantini  Egon Dejonckheere  Girts Dimdins  Yasemin Erbas  Agustín Espinosa  Gillian Finchilescu  Ángel Gómez  Roberto González  Nobuhiko Goto  Aya Hatano  Lea Hartwich  Somboon Jarukasemthawee  Jaya Kumar Karunagharan  Lindsay M. Novak  Jinseok P. Kim  Michal Kohút  Yi Liu  Steve Loughnan  Ike E. Onyishi  Charity N. Onyishi  Micaela Varela  Iris S. Pattara-angkoon  Müjde Peker  Kullaya Pisitsungkagarn  Muhammad Rizwan  Eunkook M. Suh  William Swann  Eddie M. W. Tong  Rhiannon N. Turner  Niels Vanhasbroeck  Paul A. M. Van Lange  Christin-Melanie Vauclair  Alexander Vinogradov  Grace Wacera  Zhechen Wang  Susilo Wibisono  Victoria Wai-Lan Yeung 《European journal of social psychology》2023,53(2):367-382
There is a growing body of work suggesting that social class stereotypes are amplified when people perceive higher levels of economic inequality—that is, the wealthy are perceived as more competent and assertive and the poor as more incompetent and unassertive. The present study tested this prediction in 32 societies and also examines the role of wealth-based categorization in explaining this relationship. We found that people who perceived higher economic inequality were indeed more likely to consider wealth as a meaningful basis for categorization. Unexpectedly, however, higher levels of perceived inequality were associated with perceiving the wealthy as less competent and assertive and the poor as more competent and assertive. Unpacking this further, exploratory analyses showed that the observed tendency to stereotype the wealthy negatively only emerged in societies with lower social mobility and democracy and higher corruption. This points to the importance of understanding how socio-structural features that co-occur with economic inequality may shape perceptions of the wealthy and the poor.  相似文献   
586.
587.
588.
589.
Research has examined patterns and correlates of parent/youth informant discrepancies in the reporting of youth anxiety. However, little work has examined whether it is better to conceptualize patterns and correlates of informant disagreement across anxiety broadly, or more useful to consider disagreement on specific symptom clusters. Using data from the Child Adolescent/Anxiety Multimodal Study (CAMS; N?=?488; Walkup et al. The New England Journal of Medicine, 359(26), 2753-2766. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0804633, 2008), the current study applied the most recent recommended analytic strategies to study informant discrepancies and examined differences in the magnitude and patterns of disagreement for: (a) broadband anxiety symptoms, versus (b) symptoms of specific anxiety diagnoses (or anxiety subtypes; e.g., separation, social anxiety). Correlates of informant discrepancies were also examined. Results indicated that there was variability in agreement across anxiety subtypes, with parent/youth agreement higher on separation anxiety and school refusal symptoms relative to other domains. Parental psychopathology was associated with disagreement on broadband anxiety symptoms, such that parental psychopathology was highest when parents reported higher symptoms than their children; however, this finding was largely driven by a relationship between parental psychopathology and disagreement on separation anxiety symptoms. Age was associated with disagreement on total and separation anxiety symptoms. Gender was not associated with disagreement. Clinical implications are discussed.  相似文献   
590.
Evidence from a range of fields indicates that inventions are often inspired by drawing a parallel to solutions found in nature. However, the cognitive mechanism of this process is not well understood. The cognitive mechanism of heuristic prototype in scientific innovation was tested with 3 experiments. First, 84 historical accounts of important scientific innovations were collected and the prototype solution underlying them was extracted. Experiment 1 tested the validity of these materials and showed that knowledge of the prototype facilitated insight solutions. Experiment 2 and 3 suggested that activation of the prototype was indeed the critical element in the process of insight. Also, the mechanism of this activation was linked to the semantic similarity between the feature function of the prototype and the required function of the problem. In addition, Experiment 3 showed that participants who were better at excluding interference information more readily activated the relevant prototype. To sum up, the experiments suggested that activating a memory of the heuristic prototype was important in solving scientific innovation insight problems. Activating the feature function of right heuristic prototype and linking it by way of semantic similarity to the required function of the problem was the key mechanism people used to solve scientific insight problem.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号