全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2713篇 |
免费 | 40篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
2754篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 13篇 |
2023年 | 22篇 |
2022年 | 27篇 |
2021年 | 42篇 |
2020年 | 49篇 |
2019年 | 61篇 |
2018年 | 75篇 |
2017年 | 85篇 |
2016年 | 102篇 |
2015年 | 63篇 |
2014年 | 80篇 |
2013年 | 311篇 |
2012年 | 137篇 |
2011年 | 123篇 |
2010年 | 109篇 |
2009年 | 82篇 |
2008年 | 123篇 |
2007年 | 111篇 |
2006年 | 115篇 |
2005年 | 94篇 |
2004年 | 88篇 |
2003年 | 85篇 |
2002年 | 77篇 |
2001年 | 50篇 |
2000年 | 44篇 |
1999年 | 41篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 19篇 |
1996年 | 34篇 |
1995年 | 20篇 |
1994年 | 17篇 |
1993年 | 17篇 |
1992年 | 32篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 21篇 |
1988年 | 23篇 |
1987年 | 20篇 |
1986年 | 19篇 |
1985年 | 13篇 |
1984年 | 23篇 |
1982年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 13篇 |
1979年 | 11篇 |
1975年 | 12篇 |
1974年 | 10篇 |
1973年 | 14篇 |
1972年 | 19篇 |
1971年 | 11篇 |
1969年 | 11篇 |
1968年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有2754条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
Henry J. De Haan 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1982,32(1):27-31
The relationship between subjective estimates of the comprehensibility of connected, freerunning speech and rate of speech was investigated for each of two types of time-compressed speech: pitch-varying speeded speech and pitch-normalized compressed speech. The midpoints of the resulting functions approximated the values obtained by a previously described speechrate tracking method. For equivalent degrees of comprehensibility, rates were higher for compressed speech than for speeded speech, indicating that estimates are sensitive to the intelligibility of speech. Subjective estimates of comprehensibility of time-compressed speech provide a means of assessing the itelligibility of connected speech. 相似文献
22.
De Dreu CK Beersma B Stroebe K Euwema MC 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2006,90(6):927-943
The authors tested a motivated information-processing model of negotiation: To reach high joint outcomes, negotiators need a deep understanding of the task, which requires them to exchange information and to process new information systematically. All this depends on social motivation, epistemic motivation (EM), and their interaction. Indeed, when EM (manipulated by holding negotiators process accountability or not) was high rather than low and prosocial rather than proself, negotiators recall more cooperative than competitive tactics (Experiment 1), had more trust, and reached higher joint outcomes (Experiment 2). Experiment 3 showed that under high EM, negotiators who received cooperative, rather than competitive, tactics reached higher joint outcomes because they engaged in more problem solving. Under low EM, negotiators made more concessions and reached low joint outcomes. Implications for negotiation theory and for future work in this area are discussed. 相似文献
23.
24.
Stéphanie Volders Ann Meulders Steven De Peuter Bram Vervliet Johan W.S. Vlaeyen 《Behaviour research and therapy》2012,50(11):735-746
Excessive fear of movement-related pain (FMRP), and its associated avoidance behavior, is considered a major risk factor for disability in chronic musculoskeletal pain. The current study aimed to investigate whether engaging in safety behavior, conceptualized as an avoidance response, hampers the extinction of FMRP. In a differential conditioning paradigm, we used joystick movements as conditioned stimuli (CSs) and a painful electrocutaneous stimulus as the unconditioned stimulus (US). In the Safety group, participants received the opportunity to avoid the pain-US by pressing a safety button during the extinction phase, whereas in the Control group, this option was not included. In a subsequent test phase, this safety button was no longer available. In two experiments, results demonstrate successful acquisition and extinction. Retrospective FMRP ratings in both experiments revealed a return of fear of pain in the test phase in the Safety group, but not in the Control group. In Experiment 1, mean eyeblink startle reflex amplitudes partly corroborated the self-report findings on fear of pain. The present results suggest that performing safety behavior during cognitive-behavioral interventions, i.e., exposure, might increase the risk of a return of FMRP. 相似文献
25.
26.
27.
Loneliness concerns the subjective evaluation that the number of relationships is smaller than the individual considers desirable or that the intimacy that the individual wishes for has not been realized. The aim of this study was to assess variations in levels of late-life loneliness and its determinants across Europe. Data came from the SHARE surveys, Wave 2 (Borsch-Supan et al., 2008), encompassing adults aged 50 years and over in Austria, Belgium, the Czech Republic, Denmark, France, Germany, Greece, Ireland, Italy, the Netherlands, Poland, Spain, Sweden, and Switzerland (N = 12,248). Loneliness was measured by a single item derived from the CES-D (depression) scale. Using logistic models, the present authors tested several types of explanations for country differences: differences in demographic characteristics, wealth and health, and social networks. Older adults in the southern and central European countries were generally lonelier than their peers in the northern and western European countries. In the southern and central European countries, loneliness was largely attributable to not being married, economic deprivation, and poor health. Frequent contacts with parents and adult children, social participation, and providing support to family members were important in preventing and alleviating loneliness in almost all countries. To combat loneliness among older adults, the findings suggest both (a) generic approaches aimed at improving social embeddedness and (b) country-tailored approaches aimed at improving health and wealth. 相似文献
28.
Two experiments using an immersive virtual reality foraging environment determined the spatial strategies spontaneously deployed by people in a foraging task and the effects on immediate serial recall of trajectories though the foraging space, which could conform or violate specific organisational constraints. People benefitted from the use of organised search patterns when attempting to monitor their travel though either a clustered "patchy" space or a matrix of locations. The results are discussed within a comparative framework. 相似文献
29.
Participants were presented with sentences mentioning an entity with a typical location in the upper or lower vertical space (e.g., roof vs. root). Sentences supported or reversed the typical location of the target entity. Sensibility judgments requiring upwards or downwards responses were faster when the response matched rather than mismatched the target entity's typical location. This compatibility effect was independent of whether the sentence context supported or reversed the target entity's typical location. The results, therefore, provide clear evidence for word-based but no evidence for sentence-based simulation processes. 相似文献
30.
Helen De Cruz 《Cognitive processing》2012,13(1):137-140
Several philosophical theories attempt to explain how actions performed in the world enhance cognitive processing: internalism, active externalism, and cognitive integration. The aim of this paper is to examine whether the use of spatial representations in arithmetic can shed light on this debate. Relying on philosophical analysis, on a discussion of empirical work in the cognitive neuroscience of number, and on a historical case study, I will show that spatial representations of number indicate an integration between internal and external cognitive processes. 相似文献