全文获取类型
收费全文 | 897篇 |
免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
902篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 17篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 31篇 |
2017年 | 19篇 |
2016年 | 21篇 |
2015年 | 16篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 94篇 |
2012年 | 42篇 |
2011年 | 34篇 |
2010年 | 23篇 |
2009年 | 24篇 |
2008年 | 34篇 |
2007年 | 28篇 |
2006年 | 27篇 |
2005年 | 31篇 |
2004年 | 30篇 |
2003年 | 34篇 |
2002年 | 34篇 |
2001年 | 21篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 19篇 |
1997年 | 19篇 |
1996年 | 12篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 16篇 |
1993年 | 19篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 16篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 12篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 8篇 |
1982年 | 10篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有902条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
121.
122.
May Ling Halim Diane N. Ruble Catherine S. Tamis‐LeMonda 《The British journal of developmental psychology》2013,31(1):128-135
Children's awareness of how others evaluate their gender could influence their behaviours and well‐being, yet little is known about when this awareness develops and what influences its emergence. The current study investigated culturally diverse 4‐year‐olds' (N = 240) public regard for gender groups and whether exposure to factors that convey status and highlight gender influenced it. Children were asked whether most people thought (i) girls or boys, and (ii) women or men, were better. Overall, children thought others more positively evaluated their own gender. However more TV exposure and, among girls only, more traditional parental division of housework predicted children stating that others thought boys were better, suggesting more awareness of greater male status. Children's public regard was distinct from their personal attitudes. 相似文献
123.
124.
125.
Elizabeth J. Goldman;Diane Poulin-Dubois; 《Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science》2024,15(4):e1676
This review article examines the extant literature on animism and anthropomorphism in infants and young children. A substantial body of work indicates that both infants and young children have a broad concept of what constitutes a sentient agent and react to inanimate objects as they do to people in the same context. The literature has also revealed a developmental pattern in which anthropomorphism decreases with age, but social robots appear to be an exception to this pattern. Additionally, the review shows that children attribute psychological properties to social robots less so than people but still anthropomorphize them. Importantly, some research suggests that anthropomorphism of social robots is dependent upon their morphology and human-like behaviors. The extent to which children anthropomorphize robots is dependent on their exposure to them and the presence of human-like features. Based on the existing literature, we conclude that in infancy, a large range of inanimate objects (e.g., boxes, geometric figures) that display animate motion patterns trigger the same behaviors observed in child-adult interactions, suggesting some implicit form of anthropomorphism. The review concludes that additional research is needed to understand what infants and children judge as social agents and how the perception of inanimate agents changes over the lifespan. As exposure to robots and virtual assistants increases, future research must focus on better understanding the full impact that regular interactions with such partners will have on children's anthropomorphizing. 相似文献
126.
Diane P. Berth Melanie H. Bachmeyer Caitlin A. Kirkwood Courtney R. Mauzy Billie J. Retzlaff Amanda L. Gibson 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2019,52(3):622-641
We conducted functional analyses of the inappropriate mealtime behavior of 5 children diagnosed with feeding problems. Then, we compared the effects of differential and noncontingent reinforcement, and the relative effects of escape extinction with and without differential or noncontingent reinforcement, when escape extinction appeared necessary. Both reinforcement procedures were effective without escape extinction to treat food refusal for 1 child, but only differential reinforcement was effective without escape extinction to treat the child's liquid refusal. Escape extinction was necessary for 4 of 5 children. The addition of positive reinforcement resulted in beneficial effects (i.e., more stable acceptance, decreased inappropriate mealtime behavior or negative vocalizations) with 3 of 4 children. With escape extinction, differential reinforcement was more effective to treat food refusal for 2 children and noncontingent reinforcement was more effective for 1 child. 相似文献
127.
Melanie H. Bachmeyer Caitlin A. Kirkwood Amanda B. Criscito Courtney R. Mauzy Diane P. Berth 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2019,52(3):603-621
Investigators have evaluated two procedural variations for conducting a functional analysis of inappropriate mealtime behavior exhibited by children with feeding problems. One method involves prompting bites only in the escape condition (e.g., Najdowski et al., 2008). Another method involves prompting bites across all conditions (e.g., Piazza et al., 2003). We assessed the food refusal of 3 children diagnosed with a feeding disorder by comparing the two variations. The two methods resulted in different outcomes for 2 of 3 children. Prompting bites only in the escape condition identified a single function (i.e., escape). Prompting bites across all conditions identified multiple functions (i.e., escape and attention). We then examined the relative effects of extinction procedures (individually and in combination) to determine the validity of each method. Results of the treatment evaluation suggested that the procedural variation that failed to identify an attention function for 2 of 3 children produced false negative findings. 相似文献
128.
Matthew L. Hall Inge‐Marie Eigsti Heather Bortfeld Diane Lillo‐Martin 《Developmental science》2018,21(3)
Developmental psychology plays a central role in shaping evidence‐based best practices for prelingually deaf children. The Auditory Scaffolding Hypothesis (Conway et al., 2009) asserts that a lack of auditory stimulation in deaf children leads to impoverished implicit sequence learning abilities, measured via an artificial grammar learning (AGL) task. However, prior research is confounded by a lack of both auditory and language input. The current study examines implicit learning in deaf children who were (Deaf native signers) or were not (oral cochlear implant users) exposed to language from birth, and in hearing children, using both AGL and Serial Reaction Time (SRT) tasks. Neither deaf nor hearing children across the three groups show evidence of implicit learning on the AGL task, but all three groups show robust implicit learning on the SRT task. These findings argue against the Auditory Scaffolding Hypothesis, and suggest that implicit sequence learning may be resilient to both auditory and language deprivation, within the tested limits. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at: https://youtu.be/EeqfQqlVHLI [Correction added on 07 August 2017, after first online publication: The video abstract link was added.] 相似文献
129.
Diane Brothers Cook Michallene G. McDaniel Susann M. Doyle-Portillo 《Journal of Beliefs & Values》2018,39(3):304-316
The relationship between religious orientation and personality has been investigated using several different measures and diverse samples. As part of a larger study, 114 undergraduate students were given the Duke University Religion Index (DUREL) to include only those participants who were considered religious. The final sample of 80 participants were given the NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO PI-R) and the New Indices of Religious Orientation (NIRO). Based on the results of the bivariate correlations, higher levels of intrinsic religious orientation were associated with greater Agreeableness and Conscientiousness. Multivariate analyses further revealed that Agreeableness was the most important personality variable when predicting intrinsic and quest religious orientations, with higher levels of Agreeableness predicting greater intrinsic and lower quest orientations. While lower levels of Neuroticism predicted greater intrinsic orientation, higher levels of Neuroticism predicted greater quest orientations. Higher Conscientiousness predicted greater intrinsic and lower quest orientations. Implications of these findings are discussed. 相似文献
130.
Sascha Topolinski Lea Boecker Thorsten M. Erle Giti Bakhtiari Diane Pecher 《Cognition & emotion》2017,31(1):3-18
In eight experiments, we explored matching effects between oral approach–avoidance movements triggered by word articulation and meaning of the objects the words denoted. Participants (total N?=?1264) rated their liking for words that featured consonantal muscle stricture spots either wandering inwards (e.g., BODIKA, resembling ingestion movements) or outwards (e.g., KODIBA, resembling expectoration movements). These words were labelled as names for various objects. For objects the use of which entails ingestive oral actions (lemonade and mouthwash) inward words were preferred over outward words. For objects that trigger expectorative oral actions (toxical chemical, pill, and bubble gum) this preference was attenuated or even reversed (outward words were liked more than inward). Valence of the denoted object did not play a role in these modulations. Thus, the sagittal direction of mouth movements during silent reading meaningfully interacted with direction of oral actions associated with the denoted objects. 相似文献