全文获取类型
收费全文 | 39151篇 |
免费 | 611篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
39764篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 448篇 |
2019年 | 555篇 |
2018年 | 745篇 |
2017年 | 806篇 |
2016年 | 825篇 |
2015年 | 576篇 |
2014年 | 696篇 |
2013年 | 2982篇 |
2012年 | 1285篇 |
2011年 | 1270篇 |
2010年 | 840篇 |
2009年 | 789篇 |
2008年 | 1142篇 |
2007年 | 1155篇 |
2006年 | 1027篇 |
2005年 | 915篇 |
2004年 | 858篇 |
2003年 | 815篇 |
2002年 | 833篇 |
2001年 | 1177篇 |
2000年 | 1121篇 |
1999年 | 875篇 |
1998年 | 394篇 |
1997年 | 391篇 |
1996年 | 352篇 |
1995年 | 367篇 |
1994年 | 353篇 |
1993年 | 365篇 |
1992年 | 742篇 |
1991年 | 655篇 |
1990年 | 713篇 |
1989年 | 609篇 |
1988年 | 642篇 |
1987年 | 603篇 |
1986年 | 609篇 |
1985年 | 557篇 |
1984年 | 514篇 |
1983年 | 489篇 |
1982年 | 359篇 |
1979年 | 575篇 |
1978年 | 408篇 |
1975年 | 456篇 |
1974年 | 502篇 |
1973年 | 532篇 |
1972年 | 399篇 |
1971年 | 392篇 |
1969年 | 401篇 |
1968年 | 472篇 |
1967年 | 429篇 |
1966年 | 355篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
871.
Rats received Pavlovian aversive (shock) conditioning in which white noise was established for independent groups as a CS+, CSo, or CS−. Then, in an easy (light-dark) T-maze discrimination, the CS was presented either immediately following choice (Locus 1) or at the food cup in the goal (Locus 3), contingent upon either a food-reinforced (right) or nonreinforced (wrong) response. When presented at Locus 3, the CS+ facilitated and the CS− retarded learning for CS/right subjects, with these effects being exactly reversed but somewhat less pronounced for CS/wrong subjects. However, when the CS was presented at Locus 1, the CS+ and CS− effects for both response contingencies were attenuated. These findings oppose an interpretation of the CS's function as a general cue or transformed signal for the presence or absence of the new appetitive reinforcer and argue instead for across-reinforcement blocking effects: By signaling in the presence of food reinforcement an outcome (safety or shock) which is consonant with or discrepant from the “good” outcome obtained, the CS− blocks (retards) and the CS+ counterblocks (enhances) the association of food reinforcement and the SD; conversely, in the presence of nonreinforcement (a “bad” outcome), the CS− counterblocks and the CS+ blocks inhibitory conditioning to the SΔ. As in Kamin's (1968) original research, such effects are attenuated when the CS is presented early in the compound, distal to the reinforcer. 相似文献
872.
Tucker has outlined an application of principal components analysis to a set of learning curves, for the purpose of identifying meaningful dimensions of individual differences in learning tasks. Since the principal components are defined in terms of a statistical criterion (maximum variance accounted for) rather than a substantive one, it is typically desirable to rotate the components to a more interpretable orientation. Simple structure is not a particularly appealing consideration for such a rotation; it is more reasonable to believe that any meaningful factor should form a (locally) smooth curve when the component loadings are plotted against trial number. Accordingly, this paper develops a procedure for transforming an arbitrary set of component reference curves to a new set which are mutually orthogonal and, subject to orthogonality, are as smooth as possible in a well defined (least squares) sense. Potential applications to learning data, electrophysiological responses, and growth data are indicated.Portions of this research were supported by the National Research Council of Canada, Grant A8615 to the second author. We thank Jagdeth Sheth for supplying his raw data. 相似文献
873.
874.
In deciding that two items come from the same category, subjects were delayed if the items were presented as a word and a picture, as compared with two words or two pictures. Subjects were not delayed if the items were not to be compared. Hence, the delay appears to occur during the retrieval and comparison of know ledge about the items, not during their perceptual analysis. The results suggest that there are two distinct codes involved in the categorization of words and pictures, rather than a single code. 相似文献
875.
876.
877.
L Friedman 《The Psychoanalytic quarterly》1977,46(3):425-465
A major concern of nineteenth century scientists was to delineate the objective units or entities with which science deals. While some philosophers of science abandoned the search as futile and tried to redefine science, investigators in the biological sciences and the sciences of man attempted to discover new entities upon which to base their disciplines. A new understanding of hypnotism gave Freud the opportunity to isolate objective entities within the mind and to found a psychology on that basis. 相似文献
878.
L Wurmser 《The Psychoanalytic quarterly》1977,46(3):466-498
This study of psychoanalytic theory formation is based on the epistemology of Cassirer and Langer. It postulates the need of all sciences to operate with symbols of various levels of abstractions, including, in a very prominent way, metaphors. There is not just one scientific method. The current wave of criticism against psychoanalytic theory from within and without, and especially of its use of metaphor in theory formation, is based on a philosophy of radical empiricism which cannot do justice to the science of psychoanalysis. 相似文献
879.
Jorge L. Mendoza 《Psychometrika》1977,42(1):145-147
A procedure that utilizes the sample multiple correlation to form a lower bound for the level of predictive precision of a fitted regression equation is suggested. The procedure is shown to yield probability statements which are true at least 100(1–)% of the time. 相似文献
880.