全文获取类型
收费全文 | 759篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
771篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 6篇 |
2023年 | 12篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 16篇 |
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 28篇 |
2018年 | 34篇 |
2017年 | 18篇 |
2016年 | 34篇 |
2015年 | 23篇 |
2014年 | 32篇 |
2013年 | 85篇 |
2012年 | 45篇 |
2011年 | 40篇 |
2010年 | 31篇 |
2009年 | 20篇 |
2008年 | 25篇 |
2007年 | 29篇 |
2006年 | 29篇 |
2005年 | 22篇 |
2004年 | 21篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 19篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有771条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
Diana D. Raymond Peter W. Dowrick Chris L. Kleinke 《Counselling psychology quarterly》1993,6(3):193-200
Despite the growing popularity of watching oneself on videotape, little systematic research has been conducted determine viewers' affective responses to video replay. This study addresses the question: what affective responses do women have when they view themselves for the first time on unedited videotape, in comparison to their responses when they view a peer or nature scenes on videotape? Affective responses were measured in three ways: self-report, physiological, and behavioral. Subjects who viewed themselves reported more negative feelings and anxiety, and smiled more frequently than subjects who watched another person or nature scenes on video. Subjects with high private self-consciousness had higher mean arterial blood pressures when viewing themselves than did subjects with low private self-consciousness. Overall, the implications for seeing oneself on video point to potential embarrassment but generally moderate effects. 相似文献
3.
This study analysed the way 20 managers in the information technology industry in Australia viewed their lives, and identified two dimensions of central importance cutting across the work, family and leisure spheres encompassed by them. One dimension was positively loaded towards activities which were challenging, creative and under the individual's control. These activities were enjoyable, and included examples such as consulting with clients, creating new business, and communicating and dealing with others, whether at work or at home. The opposite pole of this dimension involved activities such as completing paperwork, general administration, doing the shopping, and disciplining the children. Such activities were seen as necessary, routine or stressful, and were generally disliked. The other dimension dealt with activities which, at one pole, were emotionally involving or involved moral standards, and included examples such as attending meetings, doing things as a family, and settling disputes, either at work or at home. At the other pole were activities which were usually done alone and involved no sense of social or moral obligation. Leisure activities typically fell into this category. The results indicated that these managers generally pursued a lifestyle which was seen to be non-stressful, and one valued for the challenge, enjoyment, freedom of choice and personal control which it offered. 相似文献
4.
Enhancing possible selves in Mexican American students 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Day Jeanne D. Borkowski John G. Punzo Diana Howsepian Barbara 《Motivation and emotion》1994,18(1):79-103
This project assessed the modifiability of possible selves in young Mexican American children. Three intervention conditions were compared in a pre- and posttest design: child-only intervention, parent and child intervention, and a no-intervention control. Following eight intervention sessions, children in the two intervention groups showed significant gains in understanding the characteristics associated with becoming a good student and in recognizing the value of education in bringing about future occupational goals. Children in the intervention conditions also reported greater interest in becoming a physician, judge, or pilot — occupations they had directly experienced during training. The parent intervention phase contributed little to augmenting the positive results attributable to the child intervention component.This research was conducted under the auspices of NIH Grant HD-24003. We wish to thank Maritza Robles and the staff of the Bilingual program of the South Bend, IN, Corporation for their support. 相似文献
5.
Diana Deutsch 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1978,30(2):283-287
An experiment was conducted to investigate the effect of spatial separation on interference effects in pitch memory. Subjects compared the pitches of two tones that were separated by a sequence of eight interpolated tones. It was found that error rates were lower in sequences where the test and interpolated tones were presented to different ears, compared with sequences where they were presented to the same ear; however, this effect of spatial separation was not large. It is concluded that differences in spatial location can enable the focussing of attention away from the irrelevant tones and so reduce their disruptive effect, but that this occurs only to a limited extent. 相似文献
6.
7.
Diana S. Woodruff-Pak 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》2001,36(2):87-108
Eyeblink classical conditioning is a useful paradigm for the study of the neurobiology of learning, memory, and aging, which
also has application in the differential diagnosis of neurodegenerative diseases expressed in advancing age. Converging evidence
from studies of eyeblink conditioning in neurological patients and brain imaging in normal adults document parallels in the
neural substrates of this form of associative learning in humans and non-human mammals. Age differences in the short-delay
procedure (400 ms CS-US interval) appear in middle age in humans and may be caused at least in part by cerebellar cortical
changes such as loss of Purkinje cells. Whereas the hippocampus is not essential for conditioning in the delay procedure,
disruption of hippocampal cholinergic neurotransmission impairs acquisition and slows the rate of learning. Alzheimer’s disease
(AD) profoundly disrupts the hippocampal cholinergic system, and patients with AD consistently perform poorly in eyeblink
conditioning. We hypothesize that disruption of hippocampal cholinergic pathways in AD in addition to age-associated Purkinje
cell loss results in severely impaired eyeblink conditioning. The earliest pathology in AD occurs in entorhinal cortical input
to hippocampus, and eyeblink conditioning may detect this early disruption before declarative learning and memory circuits
become impaired. A case study is presented in which eyeblink conditioning detected impending dementia six years before changes
on other screening tests indicated impairment. Because eyeblink conditioning is simple, non-threatening, and non-invasive,
it may become a useful addition to test batteries designed to differentiate normal aging from mild cognitive impairment that
progresses to AD and AD from other types of dementia. 相似文献
8.
9.
Kristel Thomassin Diana Morelen Cynthia Suveg 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》2012,42(4):207-213
Theory and empirical research suggest that electronic diaries, which require children to report on their emotional experiences, might encourage emotional processing and facilitate a reduction in symptoms of anxiety, particularly for children exhibiting emotion-related deficits. Electronic diaries were given to 52 children (aged 7–12) to track their emotions and associated intensity for 7?days; anxiety was assessed prior to and after the use of the electronic diary by both parent- and child-report. It was hypothesized that the use of an electronic diary to track emotional experiences would lead to a reduction in anxiety symptoms for those children high in poor awareness, high in expressive reluctance, and low in emotion coping. The use of electronic diaries was effective at reducing child self-reported anxiety symptoms in girls who reported low emotion coping and reducing parent-reported anxiety for girls who reported a high reluctance to express emotions. Current findings suggest that having children identify their emotions using electronic diaries may be particularly beneficial for girls who are reluctant to express emotions and who report poor coping abilities. 相似文献
10.
Helen De Cruz 《Cognitive processing》2012,13(1):137-140
Several philosophical theories attempt to explain how actions performed in the world enhance cognitive processing: internalism, active externalism, and cognitive integration. The aim of this paper is to examine whether the use of spatial representations in arithmetic can shed light on this debate. Relying on philosophical analysis, on a discussion of empirical work in the cognitive neuroscience of number, and on a historical case study, I will show that spatial representations of number indicate an integration between internal and external cognitive processes. 相似文献