首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   8461篇
  免费   319篇
  国内免费   4篇
  8784篇
  2021年   64篇
  2020年   135篇
  2019年   148篇
  2018年   188篇
  2017年   201篇
  2016年   197篇
  2015年   182篇
  2014年   208篇
  2013年   1021篇
  2012年   334篇
  2011年   343篇
  2010年   210篇
  2009年   253篇
  2008年   355篇
  2007年   330篇
  2006年   304篇
  2005年   293篇
  2004年   290篇
  2003年   271篇
  2002年   269篇
  2001年   121篇
  2000年   116篇
  1999年   134篇
  1998年   143篇
  1997年   103篇
  1996年   103篇
  1995年   105篇
  1994年   91篇
  1993年   83篇
  1992年   87篇
  1991年   87篇
  1990年   69篇
  1989年   75篇
  1988年   74篇
  1987年   66篇
  1986年   66篇
  1985年   89篇
  1984年   85篇
  1983年   79篇
  1982年   73篇
  1981年   76篇
  1980年   74篇
  1979年   64篇
  1978年   78篇
  1977年   73篇
  1976年   87篇
  1975年   78篇
  1974年   67篇
  1973年   56篇
  1972年   50篇
排序方式: 共有8784条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
161.
Positive and negative implications of two types of college-age narcissism on psychosocial functioning at midlife were studied in a longitudinal sample of women. Both types were scored with self-report measures when the women were, on the average, age 21. Throughout the first half of their adult life, high scorers on covert narcissism presented themselves as lacking in confidence and having low morale; high scorers on overt narcissism described themselves as wanting admiration, being independent, and being forceful. Positive outcomes associated with covert narcissism included the development of insight and of a differentiated sense of the self and, in the case of overt narcissism, entailed an involvement in high-status and creative work careers. At midlife, high scorers on covert narcissism experienced negative affect and feelings of being neglected, whereas high scorers on overt narcissism experienced family conflict and problems with impulse control. The personality characteristics associated with both types of narcissism remained stable from the early 20s to early 50s.  相似文献   
162.
A series of studies investigated the capacity of children between the ages of 7 and 12 to give free and informed consent to participation in psychological research. Children were reasonably accurate in describing the purpose of studies, but many did not understand the possible benefits or especially the possible risks of participating. In several studies children's consent was not affected by the knowledge that their parents had given their permission or by the parents saying that they would not be upset if the children refused. In contrast, other studies found that children were much more likely to stop their participation if the experimenter said explicitly that she would not be upset if they stopped. We suggest that experimenters should pay more attention to describing the possible risks and benefits of participation in research, and that they should also make it clearer to children that they are free to stop once they have begun.  相似文献   
163.
164.
165.
The authors discuss the role of an academic department in promoting faculty development. Specifically, the authors (a) define faculty development from a departmental perspective, (b) discuss stages and seasons in academic careers, and (c) delineate specific faculty development goals and programs within the department.  相似文献   
166.
Is workaholism an addiction or a rewarding work style and life-style that may be harmful to one's family and co-workers? This central question directed the review of definitions and causes of workaholism. Thereafter, the authors discuss the role of “Type A” and obsessive-compulsive traits on the behavior of some workaholics, review the effects of workaholism on workaholics themselves and on others, and suggest counseling approaches that might be helpful to workaholics.  相似文献   
167.
The study examines the relative merits of a noncompatibility and a restructuring explanation of the recurrent empirical finding that a prominent attribute looms larger in choices than in judgments. Pairs of equally attractive options were presented to 72 undergraduates who were assigned to six conditions in which they performed (1) only preference judgments or choices, (2) preference judgments or choices preceded by judgments of attractiveness of attribute levels, or (3) preference judgments or choices accompanied by think-aloud reports. The results replicated the prominence effect for choices, but a prominence effect was also found for preference judgments. In accordance with the restructuring explanation, the think-aloud protocols indicated that options were more often restructured in choices than in preference judgments. However, restructuring could not explain the prominence effect observed for preference judgments. A modified compatibility hypothesis is offered as an alternative explanation.  相似文献   
168.
169.
170.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号