全文获取类型
收费全文 | 20892篇 |
免费 | 54篇 |
专业分类
20946篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 16篇 |
2019年 | 9篇 |
2018年 | 3504篇 |
2017年 | 2832篇 |
2016年 | 2263篇 |
2015年 | 206篇 |
2014年 | 90篇 |
2013年 | 119篇 |
2012年 | 589篇 |
2011年 | 2409篇 |
2010年 | 2519篇 |
2009年 | 1483篇 |
2008年 | 1737篇 |
2007年 | 2193篇 |
2006年 | 57篇 |
2005年 | 237篇 |
2004年 | 185篇 |
2003年 | 135篇 |
2002年 | 69篇 |
2001年 | 40篇 |
2000年 | 63篇 |
1999年 | 17篇 |
1998年 | 24篇 |
1997年 | 24篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1964年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
Chenyang Li 《Dao》2018,17(2):247-255
52.
53.
Robert Zaborowski 《Axiomathes》2018,28(3):253-267
It seems obvious that various feelings (various kinds of affectivity) are memorized, forgotten, and recollected to various degrees. Some of them are forgotten. Some of those forgotten can be recollected, while others are lost forever. For example, short and long-lasting feelings and shallow and deep feelings are memorized and remembered in different ways. In this paper I analyse from a conceptual point of view several categories of memory-of-feelings and offer a comprehensive map of them. In the end, the richness of categories in the realm of memory is interpreted as a proof of the intricacy of affectivity. 相似文献
54.
55.
56.
Christopher Coenen 《Nanoethics》2018,12(3):177-180
57.
Phonology, lexical access in reading, and articulatory suppression: A critical review 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Derek Besner 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1987,39(3):467-478
Considerable confusion exists in the literature on visual word recognition and reading with respect to the effects of articulatory suppression upon phonological recoding. The authors of a large number of journal articles, chapters, cognitive psychology textbooks, and books devoted to reading processes have concluded that suppression interferes with phonological receding of print and have used this supposed fact as a basis for determining when phonology is involved in various reading tasks. Others have concluded that suppression need not interfere with phonological recoding (e.g. Besner, Davies and Daniels, 1981; Besner and Davelaar, 1982). The present review concludes that a phonological code can be derived from printed English and used for lexical access without interference from suppression. However, operations performed upon a phonological code—e.g. post-assembly phonemic segmentation and deletion, maintenance in working memory—are disrupted by suppression. A review of the literature supports this distinction; some implications of these views are noted. 相似文献
58.
In this paper, we consider the different methods that have been developed to quantify random generation behavior and incorporate these measurement scales into a Windows95 computer program called RgCalc. RgCalc analyzes the quality of human attempts at random generation and can provide computer-generated, pseudorandom sequences for comparison. The program is designed to be appropriate for the analysis of various types of random generation situations employed in the psychological literature. The different algorithms for the evaluation of a dataset are detailed and an outline of the program is described. Performance measures are available for assessing various aspects of the response distribution, the sequencing of pairs, the ordinal relationships between sets of items, and the tendency to repeat alternatives over different lengths. A factor analysis is used to illustrate the multiple dimensions underlying human randomization processes. 相似文献
59.
Stimulus repetition facilitates performance in many experimental contexts. However, an episodic approach to interpreting repetition effects suggests that repetition effects should depend on how stimuli are encoded. In Experiments 1-3, stimulus repetition in a word identification task led to a cost rather than a benefit in performance, but only when the prime was presented for a relatively long duration. One account of these results is that long prime durations allow integration between stimulus and response codes to occur, which in turn can interfere with responding to a following identical target. In Experiment 4, a stimulus intensity based episodic specificity effect that was insensitive to the proportion of repeated trials supported this stimulus-response integration account. 相似文献
60.
It has been argued that the psychological refractory period (PRP) effect is eliminated with two ideomotor compatible tasks when instructions stress fast and simultaneous responding. Three experiments were conducted to test this hypothesis. In all experiments, Task 1 required spatially compatible manual responses (left or right) to the direction of an arrow, and Task 2 required saying the name of the auditory letter A or B. In Experiments 1 and 3, the manual responses were keypresses made with the left and right hands, whereas in Experiment 2 they were left-right toggle-switch movements made with the dominant hand. Instructions that stressed response speed reduced reaction time and increased error rate compared to standard instructions to respond fast and accurately, but did not eliminate the PRP effect on Task 2 reaction time. These results imply that, even when response speed is emphasized, ideomotor compatible tasks do not bypass response selection. 相似文献