全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2336篇 |
免费 | 106篇 |
专业分类
2442篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 20篇 |
2020年 | 36篇 |
2019年 | 32篇 |
2018年 | 43篇 |
2017年 | 68篇 |
2016年 | 56篇 |
2015年 | 38篇 |
2014年 | 43篇 |
2013年 | 246篇 |
2012年 | 82篇 |
2011年 | 92篇 |
2010年 | 55篇 |
2009年 | 57篇 |
2008年 | 80篇 |
2007年 | 78篇 |
2006年 | 70篇 |
2005年 | 65篇 |
2004年 | 64篇 |
2003年 | 69篇 |
2002年 | 58篇 |
2001年 | 59篇 |
2000年 | 72篇 |
1999年 | 53篇 |
1998年 | 41篇 |
1997年 | 31篇 |
1996年 | 30篇 |
1995年 | 34篇 |
1994年 | 24篇 |
1993年 | 24篇 |
1992年 | 32篇 |
1991年 | 35篇 |
1990年 | 30篇 |
1989年 | 28篇 |
1988年 | 24篇 |
1987年 | 37篇 |
1986年 | 23篇 |
1985年 | 28篇 |
1984年 | 28篇 |
1983年 | 29篇 |
1982年 | 29篇 |
1981年 | 27篇 |
1980年 | 33篇 |
1979年 | 33篇 |
1978年 | 25篇 |
1977年 | 28篇 |
1976年 | 30篇 |
1975年 | 25篇 |
1974年 | 30篇 |
1971年 | 19篇 |
1966年 | 19篇 |
排序方式: 共有2442条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
Kevin C. Mooney Robert Thompson James M. Nelson 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1987,15(1):67-73
The relationship between risk factors and the severity and type of childhood disorder, as measured by parent-completed Child Behavior Checklists, was examined for 768 children, ages 4–16, seen at a child mental health center. Regression analyses revealed no significant relationships between any combination of risk factors and the total number of behavior problems, internalizing, or externalizing scores for the entire group, males and females separately, or for the age and sex groupings of 6- to 11- and 12-to 16-yearolds. Chi-square tests revealed no relationship of profile type with any risk factor or with total number of risk factors. Implications for broad-based child assessment and risk factor research within a clinical population are discussed.We wish to thank Orin Bolstad, Linda Magnuson, and Julian Taplin for their assistance in this project. 相似文献
12.
13.
To determine whether actual responses of potential comforters in the community differ according to cause of death, 83 college students participated in a structured, individual interview. They were asked demographic questions about themselves, the bereaved, and the deceased, and then about various aspects of how they, and others in the community, responded to the death. Students were grouped by their reports of the cause of death (suicide, homicide, accident, natural anticipated death, or natural unanticipated death). When the death was by suicide or homicide, others were perceived as relatively less supportive of the bereaved person. When the death was by suicide, respondents themselves tended more to blame the bereaved person. When the death was by homicide, the bereaved person was perceived as reacting relatively worse. Potential comforters were relatively more shocked when the death was by homicide or accident. 相似文献
14.
McIntire KD Cleary J Thompson T 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1987,47(3):279-285
Two cynomolgous macaques categorized six colors into two groups of three after conditional discrimination training (zero-delay symbolic match-to-sample). The procedures resulted in the establishment of relations among the elements of each set-relations that were not specifically trained and that can be characterized by the properties of reflexivity, symmetry, and transitivity. Each set of colors was related to a characteristic pattern of responding: One response pattern involved temporal duration (press and hold the response keys); the second response pattern entailed repeated pressing and releasing of the response keys (fixed ratio 8). Six combinations of two colors were trained, three combinations from each set. After discriminative performance stabilized for each monkey, they were tested with 10 additional color combinations, all of which differed from the training combinations. The conditional relations established between test combinations can be characterized as stimulus equivalence. The training procedures were analogous to the procedure of using category names, and have implications for understanding the function of language in the formation of equivalence classes. 相似文献
15.
16.
The present study examined observers’ ability to discriminate canonical and dynamically anomalous collisions that were presented in either frictionless or frictional systems. Whereas previous research has provided qualitative demonstrations that dynamic information can be extracted from visual events, the current study provides a parametric assessment of observers’ sensitivity to dynamic invariants. Our findings indicate that observers are competent when viewing both familiar, terrestrial (frictional) systems and unfamiliar but computationally simpler, 0-G (frictionless) systems. Thus, our sensitivity to these dynamic invariants in visual events is robust in natural systems whose dynamic properties differ from those of the environment in which we evolved and developed. 相似文献
17.
All seven variables of containment theory were operationalized for questionnaires. Data were collected from 677 adolescents in a variety of high schools and correctional institutions, and divided into categories by race and sex. Factor analysis indicated that the structure of the relationships among the seven containment variables and delinquency was substantially different among black females. 相似文献
18.
19.
It is suggested that molar streams of behavior are constructed of various arrangements of three elementary constituents (elicited, evoked, and emitted response classes). An eight-cell taxonomy is elaborated as a framework for analyzing and synthesizing complex behavioral repertoires based on these functional units. It is proposed that the local force binding functional units into a smoothly articulated kinetic sequence arises from temporally arranged relative response probability relationships. Behavioral integration is thought to reflect the joint influence of the organism's hierarchy of relative response probabilities, fluctuating biological states, and the arrangement of environmental and behavioral events in time. 相似文献
20.
The absence of operational disaggregate lexicographic decision models and Tversky's observation that choice behavior is often inconsistent, hierarchical, and context dependent motivate the development of a maximum likelihood hierarchical (MLH) choice model. This new disaggregate choice model requires few assumptions and accommodates the three aspects of choice behavior noted by A. Tversky (1972, Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 9, 341–367). The model has its foundation in a prototype model developed by the authors. Unlike the deterministic prototype, however, MLH is a probabilistic model which generates maximum likelihood estimators of the aggregate “cutoff values.” The model is formulated as a concave programming problem whose solutions are therefore globally optimal. Finally, the model is applied to data from three separate studies where it is demonstrated to have superior performance over the prototype model in its predictive performance. 相似文献