全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3867篇 |
免费 | 35篇 |
专业分类
3902篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 40篇 |
2023年 | 46篇 |
2022年 | 34篇 |
2021年 | 61篇 |
2020年 | 78篇 |
2019年 | 123篇 |
2018年 | 141篇 |
2017年 | 147篇 |
2016年 | 164篇 |
2015年 | 129篇 |
2014年 | 114篇 |
2013年 | 425篇 |
2012年 | 217篇 |
2011年 | 216篇 |
2010年 | 127篇 |
2009年 | 112篇 |
2008年 | 159篇 |
2007年 | 158篇 |
2006年 | 121篇 |
2005年 | 154篇 |
2004年 | 149篇 |
2003年 | 130篇 |
2002年 | 95篇 |
2001年 | 66篇 |
2000年 | 65篇 |
1999年 | 43篇 |
1998年 | 44篇 |
1997年 | 31篇 |
1996年 | 30篇 |
1995年 | 26篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 25篇 |
1992年 | 23篇 |
1991年 | 30篇 |
1990年 | 37篇 |
1989年 | 22篇 |
1988年 | 27篇 |
1987年 | 23篇 |
1986年 | 28篇 |
1985年 | 23篇 |
1984年 | 24篇 |
1983年 | 19篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 13篇 |
1978年 | 14篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1968年 | 7篇 |
1967年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有3902条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
151.
152.
Charles Abraham Paschal Sheeran Paul Norman Mark Conner Nanne de Vries Wilma Otten 《Journal of applied social psychology》1999,29(12):2591-2612
Measures of intention usually leave substantial proportions of the variance in behavior unexplained. It has been suggested that improved behavioral prediction could be achieved by identifying postdecisional cognitive processes capable of distinguishing between intenders who do act and those do not act. Condom-related self-report measures of postdecisional cognitive processes were developed and tested in a cross-sectional questionnaire study involving 447 heterosexual students. A discriminant function composed of postdecisional measures was found to significantly distinguish between intenders who reported use and non-use and to correctly classify 80% of intenders. The results indicate that measures of the relative importance of competing intentions, prior planning of specific preparatory actions, and action-specific self-efficacy may enhance the prediction of condom use among intenders. 相似文献
153.
154.
We report investigations of the face processing abilities of SP, a right-handed woman who had suffered a subarachnoid haemorrhage from a right middle cerebral artery aneurysm. Although she could correctly assign visual stimuli to the 'face' category without difficulty, SP performed poorly on all other face processing, tasks, including 'closure' (Mooney faces), perception of facial expression, unfamiliar face matching, and identification of familiar faces. Identification of familiar people from nonfacial cues (names) remained relatively well-preserved, but severe impairments were evident on all face recognition tasks. Her errors mostly involved either failures to find a face familiar at all, or misidentification as another familiar person. In face-name learning tasks, there was evidence of 'covert' recognition of faces she failed to recognize overtly. SP's face processing impairment remained stable across a 20-month period of investigation, yet throughout this period she did not think that she had any problems in face recognition, and continued to show lack of insight into this impairment even when directly confronted with its consequences on formal testing. In contrast, SP showed adequate insight into other physical and cognitive impairments produced by her illness, including poor memory, hemiplegia, and hemianopia. We propose that her lack of insight into her face recognition problems involves a deficit-specific anosognosia, resulting from impairment of domain-specific monitoring abilities. 相似文献
155.
156.
James R. Anderson Michel Anthouard Mich le de Monte Jules Kempf 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology》1993,46(4):391-398
Two groups of stumptailed macaques (Macaca arctoides) were compared in terms of performance on a visuospatial working memory task involving repeated presentations of an array of 15 doors, each of which concealed one food reward at the start of a session. Rewards obtained by the subject were not replaced. Errors were defined as responses to doors previously pushed open during the session. Both groups generally obtained all rewards and showed improved performance across sessions, but old monkeys consistently made more errors than young ones, especially in the latter half of a given session. Further, old monkeys were less diverse in the errors they made, indicating different response strategies by the two groups. 相似文献
157.
158.
In this study, cross-cultural differences in cognitive test scores are hypothesized to depend on a test's cultural complexity (Cultural Complexity Hypothesis: CCH), here conceptualized as its content familiarity, rather than on its cognitive complexity (Spearman's Hypothesis: SH). The content familiarity of tests assessing short-term memory, attention, working memory, and figural and verbal fluid reasoning, was manipulated by constructing test versions with an item content derived from either Afrikaans or Tswana culture in South Africa. Both test versions were administered to children of both cultures. The sample consisted of 161 urban Afrikaans, 181 urban, and 159 rural Tswana children (Mage = 9.37 years). Children generally performed best on the test version that was designed for their own group, particularly on the cognitively and culturally complex working memory and figural fluid reasoning tests. This relation between content familiarity and cognitive test performance supports CCH and disconfirms SH. 相似文献
159.
We set up a mutual help network among the elderly residents of a government-subsidized apartment building and evaluated its impact on their social ties, social support satisfaction, and depression. A quasi-experimental design was used, with the residents of a similar building located in the same neighborhood serving as the control group. A total of 230 individual services were exchanged, and 28 group activities were organized during the study period. There was almost no difference between experimental and control groups with respect to the change over the study period in the number of social ties with other residents of the building. Support satisfaction decreased in both groups, but the decrease was larger in the experimental group. The control group had a slight decrease in the frequency of their depressive symptoms, whereas the experimental group had a moderate increase. More research is needed on the ability of social support interventions to produce beneficial effects on mental health among individuals who are not experiencing a particular life stress. 相似文献
160.
Polman H Orobio de Castro B Koops W van Boxtel HW Merk WW 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2007,35(4):522-535
The present meta-analytic review aimed to clarify divergent findings concerning the relation between reactive and proactive aggression in children and adolescents. Fifty-one studies with 17,965 participants were included in the analysis. A significant correlation between reactive and proactive aggression was found. The strength of this relation varied considerably between studies, from -.10 to .89. Observational assessment and tilt/noise tasks were associated with smaller correlations than questionnaires. Within the large group of questionnaire studies, studies disentangling the form and function of aggression found lower correlations than studies that did not disentangle form and function. Among questionnaire studies, higher reliability was associated with larger correlations. Effect size did not depend on other study characteristics such as sample type, age, and informant type. It is concluded that reactive and proactive aggression are most clearly distinguished with behavioral observations and questionnaires that unravel form and functions of aggression. 相似文献