全文获取类型
收费全文 | 9630篇 |
免费 | 1008篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
10643篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 62篇 |
2020年 | 145篇 |
2019年 | 159篇 |
2018年 | 196篇 |
2017年 | 206篇 |
2016年 | 252篇 |
2015年 | 186篇 |
2014年 | 178篇 |
2013年 | 886篇 |
2012年 | 350篇 |
2011年 | 429篇 |
2010年 | 324篇 |
2009年 | 427篇 |
2008年 | 375篇 |
2007年 | 444篇 |
2006年 | 334篇 |
2005年 | 331篇 |
2004年 | 371篇 |
2003年 | 387篇 |
2002年 | 316篇 |
2001年 | 250篇 |
2000年 | 166篇 |
1999年 | 215篇 |
1998年 | 203篇 |
1997年 | 164篇 |
1996年 | 195篇 |
1995年 | 169篇 |
1994年 | 183篇 |
1993年 | 152篇 |
1992年 | 193篇 |
1991年 | 150篇 |
1990年 | 145篇 |
1989年 | 117篇 |
1988年 | 97篇 |
1987年 | 107篇 |
1986年 | 91篇 |
1985年 | 84篇 |
1984年 | 92篇 |
1983年 | 80篇 |
1982年 | 93篇 |
1981年 | 76篇 |
1980年 | 68篇 |
1979年 | 81篇 |
1978年 | 75篇 |
1976年 | 70篇 |
1972年 | 60篇 |
1968年 | 67篇 |
1966年 | 55篇 |
1964年 | 81篇 |
1963年 | 75篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
Abstract: Do memories change as we acquire new information? Recent research on memory distortion using implicit tests along with research using confidence is reviewed and new studies are presented. Two new studies asked misinformed subjects to provide reasons for their answers. In each study 15% to 27% of subjects said they remembered seeing items they had only read about. In another study subjects were asked to identify the source of misleading items they had seen in slides or read in misleading questions. Subjects were more likely to say they had seen in slides something they read about in the questions than they were to confuse information from two nearly identical sets of slides. Recent work shows that, not only is it possible to distort memory for events, it is possible to implant an entire memory for something that never happened. The evidence is now clear that we can become mentally tricked into making large as well as small changes in the way we recall the past. 相似文献
132.
133.
Cathleen C. Piazza Wayne W. Fisher Gregory P. Hanley Kellie Hilker K. Mark Derby 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1996,29(2):137-152
In the current investigation, a modification was made to the preference assessment described by Pace, Ivancic, Edwards, Iwata, and Page (1985) to predict the effects of stimuli when used in a differential-reinforcement-of-other-behavior (DRO) schedule for 2 clients with severe self-injurious behavior (SIB) and profound mental retardation. Based on the results of the preference assessment, three types of stimuli were identified: (a) high-preference stimuli associated with high rates of SIB (HP/HS), (b) high-preference stimuli associated with relatively lower rates of SIB (HP/LS), and (c) low-preference stimuli associated with low rates of SIB (LP/LS). Consistent with the results of the preference assessment, the DRO schedule with HP/HS stimuli resulted in increased SIB, and the DRO schedule with LP/LS stimuli resulted in no changes in SIB. HP/LS stimuli were demonstrated reinforcers but did not result in a change in SIB when used in a DRO schedule. Thus, the stimulus preference assessment may be useful clinically in some situations for predicting both the beneficial and the negative side effects of stimuli in DRO procedures. 相似文献
134.
Dianne Parker Stephen G. Stradling Antony S. R. Manstead 《Journal of applied social psychology》1996,26(1):1-19
This study builds on previous theory of planned behavior (TPB) studies in which we identified the beliefs and values which predict intention to commit driving violations. Four short experimental videos were developed in order to assess the effectiveness of an intervention grounded in the TPB. Three of the videos featured the major constructs of the TPB model. The fourth video featured anticipated regret, an addition to the TPB model which had previously been shown to add significantly to its predictive performance (Parker, Manstead, & Stradling, 1995). Results indicated that two of the videos brought about statistically significant belief changes with respect to scores on TPB items, and significant changes in general attitudes toward speeding. Discussion centers on the problems encountered in operationalizing the TPB constructs and on the potential of theory-based interventions to induce attitude change. 相似文献
135.
A sample of 171 members of the Association for Family Therapy (UK) was surveyed and data obtained on their general level of experienced stress, the sources and levels of experienced stresses they encountered in their practica, and the professional and private coping strategies they used to mitigate the effects of these stressors. The results indicate that stress can be experienced in all practice contexts, with important factors in its overall configuration being personal/professional worth and agency expectation. Family therapists appear to prefer coping strategies of a more social nature and there is an ambivalent attitude towards'therapy for therapists'. 相似文献
136.
Carol M. Trivette Carl J. Dunst Deborah Hamby 《American journal of community psychology》1996,24(2):273-293
Relationships between human services program models and help-giving practices, and between both program models and help-giving practices and help-seeker control appraisals were examined in a study of 107 low socioeconomic background families. Three kinds of human services agencies were included in the study, each of which differed in terms of their implicit and explicit assumptions about the families they served and the roles professionals and families played as part of helping relationships. Results support the hypotheses that (a) a program philosophy that was family centered would be associated with more empowering help-giving practices compared to more professionally centered philosophies and (b) a family-centered program philosophy and empowering help-giving practices would be related to an enhanced sense of personal control over needed supports and resources from a target help-giver. 相似文献
137.
The internal structure of the Repression-Sensitization (R-S) Scale was examined by means of factor analysis. Following a scree
test, four factors were extracted: lassitude/depression, extraversion/confidence, guilelessness/trust, and emotional lability.
Concurrent validation using the Emotion Control Questionnaire and the Eysenck Personality Inventory showed that the factors
extracted from the R-S Scale were related in predictable ways to existing personality constructs, but the results argue against
the use of the overall R-S score as an index of emotional response style. 相似文献
138.
The qualitative nature of the incidental memory trace produced by perceptual and conceptual processing within a speeded inference task was examined. Performance on recall and auditory recognition tests replicated the general finding that semantic processing leads to better retention of words than does nonsemantic processing. This pattern of results was reversed on a visual recognition test designed to measure the amount of perceptual information remembered. These data suggest that different types of processing result in different aspects of the stimulus being encoded, with conceptual processing resulting primarily in the encoding of semantic information and perceptual processing resulting primarily in the encoding of physical information. Thus, the effectiveness of a particular kind of processing for good memory performance depends on the kind of information being tested. 相似文献
139.
140.