首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4239篇
  免费   173篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2023年   47篇
  2022年   47篇
  2021年   59篇
  2020年   97篇
  2019年   90篇
  2018年   142篇
  2017年   151篇
  2016年   154篇
  2015年   121篇
  2014年   131篇
  2013年   448篇
  2012年   241篇
  2011年   188篇
  2010年   129篇
  2009年   123篇
  2008年   182篇
  2007年   201篇
  2006年   144篇
  2005年   131篇
  2004年   137篇
  2003年   131篇
  2002年   117篇
  2001年   79篇
  2000年   81篇
  1999年   70篇
  1998年   39篇
  1997年   44篇
  1996年   37篇
  1995年   39篇
  1994年   30篇
  1993年   36篇
  1992年   42篇
  1991年   37篇
  1990年   44篇
  1989年   32篇
  1988年   30篇
  1987年   43篇
  1986年   32篇
  1985年   33篇
  1984年   38篇
  1983年   32篇
  1981年   24篇
  1979年   37篇
  1978年   21篇
  1976年   25篇
  1975年   25篇
  1974年   24篇
  1973年   18篇
  1970年   19篇
  1968年   18篇
排序方式: 共有4414条查询结果,搜索用时 78 毫秒
911.
912.
Two studies examined mother's labeling of gender-neutral characters in young children's picture books. In the first study, mothers and their 18- to 38-month-old children looked at three popular children's books together. The mothers' use of masculine or feminine names or pronouns to refer to gender-neutral characters was recorded. The data revealed an extreme masculine bias: 95% of all characters of indeterminate gender were referred to by the mothers as males. In a second study, specially prepared picture books were used to examine the effect of gender-relevant variables on the mothers' labeling. The results replicated Study 1 in showing a strong bias toward referring to neutral pictures with masculine labels: The incidence of masculine labels was again high and, in addition, was relatively impervious to most of the gender-relevant manipulations included in the books. The one variable that had a strong, consistent effect on the mothers' labeling was the presence of an adult in the pictures: Child Characters were nearly always referred to as males when they appeared alone; they were more likely to be given feminine or neutral lables when they were pictured in the presence of an adult. This result is related to Carpenter's [Activity Structure and Play: Implications for Socialization, in M. Liss (Ed.), Social and Cognitive Skills: Sex Roles and Childrens Play, New York: Academic Press, 1983] model of the development of sex-typed behavior. The data are also discussed with respect to the well-documented sexist bias in picture books for preschool children.This research was support in part by Grants HD-05951 from NICHHD and R01 MH33082-01 from NIMH to the first and third authors, respectively. We wish to thank Linda Roots and Elizabeth Lewis for their assitance with data analyses.  相似文献   
913.
Computerphobia     
Five studies of over 450 university students presented a comprehensive picture of computer-phobia. Three research and clinically based self-report instruments were developed to measure three nearly independent dimensions of computerphobia-computer anxiety, computer attitudes, and computer cognitions and feelings. Results indicated that older students were more computer anxious, but did not have more negative attitudes, cognitions, or feelings, than did younger students. Women had more negative attitudes than did men. Feminine-identity students showed more anxiety and negative attitudes than did masculine-identity students, regardless of gender. White students had more anxiety and more positive attitudes than did nonwhite students. Computerphobia was related to other anxiety measures (mathematics, state, and trait), but was a separate construct. Experience with computer interaction did not reduce anxiety or improve attitudes. Finally, regardless of academic major, computer-anxious students showed less computer aptitude, literacy, and interest. Implications for treating computerphobia are discussed.  相似文献   
914.
The program development and first-year results of a 3-year U.S. Department of Education Fund for the Improvement of Postsecondary Education (FIPSE) grant are described. The identification of computerphobics and the types of discomfort they experience are defined. A clinically based 5-week model computerphobia reduction and skills-acquisition program is presented. Screening and assessment measures are outlined and three treatment modules—two individual treatments (systematic desensitization and thought stopping) and an information/support group—are described. The first-year results demonstrate significant pretreatment-to-posttreatment change in anxiety, attitudes, cognitions, and feelings. Plans are discussed for further evaluation and program expansion.  相似文献   
915.
The use of glass slides coated with the cationic polymer polydimethyldiallylammonium-chlorid for cell enrichment procedures results in an increasing number of cells on the slicks and changed quantitative results of cell differentiation. These investigations are important for quantitative analysis of cell populations.  相似文献   
916.
917.
To study the mechanisms underlying covert orienting of attention in visual space, subjects were given advance cues indicating the probable locations of targets that they had to discriminate and localize. Direct peripheral cues (brightening of one of four boxes in peripheral vision) and symbolic central cues (an arrow at the fixation point indicating a probable peripheral box) were compared. Peripheral and central cues are believed to activate different reflexive and voluntary modes of orienting (Jonides, 1981; Posner, 1980). Experiment 1 showed that the time courses of facilitation and inhibition from peripheral and central cues were characteristic and different. Experiment 2 showed that voluntary orienting in response to symbolic central cues is interrupted by reflexive orienting to random peripheral flashes. Experiment 3 showed that irrelevant peripheral flashes also compete with relevant peripheral cues. The amount of interference varied systematically with the interval between the onset of the relevant cue and of the distracting flash (cue-flash onset asynchrony) and with the cuing condition. Taken together, these effects support a model for spatial attention with distinct but interacting reflexive and voluntary orienting mechanisms.  相似文献   
918.
Dark vergence depends on the vertical direction of gaze; it decreases with raised gaze and increases with lowered gaze. The vertical direction of gaze can be varied by means or raising or lowering the eyes or by way of tilting the head forward or backward. The effects of both manipulations on dark vergence are different. According to Heuer (1988) the effects of head tilt and eye inclination on dark vergence are almost, but not exactly, additive. In Exp. 1 the hypothesis of additive effects of gaze direction and eye inclination was tested and could not be rejected. The two additive hypotheses (head tilt and eye inclination vs. gaze inclination and eye inclination) result in different predictions for dark vergence with "compensatory" head and eye inclinations, which leave the direction of gaze in space invariant. In Exp. 2 it was shown that predictions from both hypotheses deviated from the observed values of dark vergence. Thus none of the two additive hypotheses provides exact predictions of dark vergence for all possible combinations of head tilt and eye inclination. For practical purposes the approximation might be sufficient. In particular, although mean dark vergence cannot be predicted exactly, individual differences can be predicted quite accurately.  相似文献   
919.
It has been shown that the scent of mice changes after a bone marrow transplantation. Previously obtained results were re-examined with a new design that rules out possible avoidance learning effects. Two BALB/c mice, deprived of water for 20 hours each day, were trained in a Y-maze to discriminate two fully allogeneic mice strains (C3H = S+ and C57 = S-) via their urine odor. Reinforcement for correct choice was provided by a drop of water. Urine samples of different fully allogeneic mice strains (C3H, C57 and A/J) and bone marrow transplanted chimeras (C57----C3H and C3H----C57) were submitted to different "transfer of training" -tests. In the conditioning paradigm used, the animals learn to identify S+ and also do not avoid S- after the training. The urine odor of the C57----C3H-chimeras was found to be different from the strain-specific urine odor of C3H animals. Since this change could not be found in syngeneic transplanted chimeras, it is concluded that it is caused by the graft. The experiments failed, however to demonstrate that the urine odor of allogeneic chimeras is constituted solely by donor- and recipient-specific components.  相似文献   
920.
Katz (1925) has argued that the sense of vibration underlies the tactual perception of roughness. However, Taylor and Lederman (1975) have suggested that vibration serves only to prevent the cessation of mechanoreceptor activity. In an experimental evaluation of these positions, it is shown that, although prior (selective) adaptation of the fingertip strongly affects the perceived magnitude of supraliminal vibrotactile signals, it fails to alter the perceived roughness of metal gratings. The results thus favor the Taylor and Lederman position. The paper also speculates on roughness coding by the mechanoreceptor populations present in glabrous skin of the human hand.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号