全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1439篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
1451篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 6篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 18篇 |
2019年 | 37篇 |
2018年 | 29篇 |
2017年 | 36篇 |
2016年 | 43篇 |
2015年 | 34篇 |
2014年 | 39篇 |
2013年 | 133篇 |
2012年 | 58篇 |
2011年 | 55篇 |
2010年 | 33篇 |
2009年 | 30篇 |
2008年 | 60篇 |
2007年 | 68篇 |
2006年 | 64篇 |
2005年 | 50篇 |
2004年 | 66篇 |
2003年 | 76篇 |
2002年 | 62篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 21篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 26篇 |
1996年 | 22篇 |
1995年 | 26篇 |
1994年 | 17篇 |
1993年 | 23篇 |
1992年 | 24篇 |
1991年 | 21篇 |
1990年 | 14篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 14篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 15篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 11篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 12篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有1451条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Beauchaine TP Strassberg Z Kees MR Drabick DA 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2002,30(1):89-101
Cognitive response repertoires to videotaped child noncompliance episodes were examined in mothers of aggressive (MAs) and nonaggressive 4–6-year-old boys. Mothers provided open-ended solutions to three subtypes of child noncompliance under conditions of time pressure, or after they waited for 15 s to consider alternatives. Solutions were coded as assistance/facilitation, coercion, deference, or explanation/clarification. Compared with controls, MAs offered fewer explanation/clarification responses, more coercive responses, and fewer unique solutions during pressured responding. Two to 6 weeks later, mothers were videotaped while participating with their sons in a challenging block-building task. Maternal responses to the vignettes predicted conflict escalation during block building, even after rates of concurrent and past child noncompliance were partialled out. Implications for parent-training models are considered. 相似文献
992.
MR imaging of fetal central nervous system abnormalities 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Levine D 《Brain and cognition》2002,50(3):39-448
Ultrasound is the screening modality of choice for fetal imaging. However, when additional information regarding fetal anatomy or pathology is needed, fast MR imaging is increasingly being used as a correlative imaging modality. Performances of high quality sonographic and MR examinations as well as interpretation by individuals familiar with prenatal diagnosis are important factors for optimal parental counseling. MR imaging frequently adds additional information regarding fetal central nervous system anomalies beyond that available with ultrasound. This information commonly changes patient counseling, and at times patient management. 相似文献
993.
This study evaluated women's understanding of prenatal ultrasound in terms of meeting the requirements for informed choice. A cross-sectional survey was conducted to evaluate (1) how information is provided, (2) women's perceived value of the information received and, (3) their understanding of ultrasound in relation to the principles of informed choice. Women (n = 113) completed a questionnaire prior to their 18-week ultrasound. Fifty-five percent stated they received no information from their care provider. Only 31.9% considered health care providers as a very helpful source of information. Yet, 69.0% stated their care provider gave them information that facilitated their understanding. Gaps were identified in women's understanding of ultrasound. Specifically, 46.0% did not view ultrasound as a screen for anomalies; some were uncertain about the safety (18.6%), diagnostic capabilities (26.5%), and limitations of testing (37.2%). These results suggest that women's understanding of ultrasound does not meet the requirements of informed choice. 相似文献
994.
From power to action 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Three experiments investigated the hypothesis that power increases an action orientation in the power holder, even in contexts where power is not directly experienced. In Experiment 1, participants who possessed structural power in a group task were more likely to take a card in a simulated game of blackjack than those who lacked power. In Experiment 2, participants primed with high power were more likely to act against an annoying stimulus (a fan) in the environment, suggesting that the experience of power leads to the performance of goal-directed behavior. In Experiment 3, priming high power led to action in a social dilemma regardless of whether that action had prosocial or antisocial consequences. The effects of priming power are discussed in relation to the broader literature on conceptual and mind-set priming. 相似文献
995.
Buehner MJ Cheng PW Clifford D 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2003,29(6):1119-1140
How humans infer causation from covariation has been the subject of a vigorous debate, most recently between the computational causal power account (P. W. Cheng, 1997) and associative learning theorists (e.g., K. Lober & D. R. Shanks, 2000). Whereas most researchers in the subject area agree that causal power as computed by the power PC theory offers a normative account of the inductive process. Lober and Shanks, among others, have questioned the empirical validity of the theory. This article offers a full report and additional analyses of the original study featured in Lober and Shanks's critique (M. J. Buehner & P. W. Cheng, 1997) and reports tests of Lober and Shanks's and other explanations of the pattern of causal judgments. Deviations from normativity, including the outcome-density bias, were found to be misperceptions of the input or other artifacts of the experimental procedures rather than inherent to the process of causal induction. 相似文献
996.
997.
Adolescent development involves progressive changes in brain structure and cognitive function, but relatively few studies have documented the cognitive correlates of differences in structural brain volumes in this age group. We examined the relations among age, cognitive processing, and mesial temporal lobe volume in 37 children and adolescents. Participants completed a brief cognitive assessment battery and underwent volumetric structural magnetic resonance imaging. For the sample as a whole, amygdala volume correlated positively with age, and larger volumes of both the left and right amygdala were significantly associated with better performance on several cognitive tasks assessing academic skills and acquired knowledge in long-term memory. In contrast, hippocampal volumes did not correlate with adolescents' age and were less frequently correlated with cognitive performance. Amygdala volumes were most predictive of cognitive abilities in boys, whereas for girls, the volume of the hippocampus contributed more frequently to the prediction of cognitive abilities. These data suggest that measurable differences in mesial temporal volumes during adolescence are reliably associated with long-term cognitive abilities, particularly academic skills and the acquisition of intellectual knowledge, and that these relationships may differ as a function of the sex of the child. 相似文献
998.
Hennessy MB Young TL O'Leary SK Maken DS 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2003,117(4):406-413
Preference tests in a novel environment (Experiment 1) and unobtrusive observations in a specialized living environment (Experiment 2) examined the attractiveness of various classes of conspecifics for maturing guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus). It was found that (a). the young continued to remain near the mother well beyond weaning; (b). there was increased time spent with unrelated adult females, but not males, after weaning; (c). male and female offspring behaved similarly; and (d). littermates spent considerable time with each other. These results provide no evidence that guinea pigs approaching sexual maturity begin to associate preferentially with novel animals or potential breeding partners. Choices were largely predictable from earlier findings of the ability of various classes of conspecifics to reduce hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity of the young. 相似文献
999.
1000.
Rocco Pallin Bonnie Siry Deborah Azrael Christopher E. Knoepke Daniel D. Matlock Ashley Clement Megan L. Ranney Garen J. Wintemute Marian E. Betz 《Behavioral sciences & the law》2019,37(3):259-269
A recommended component of suicide prevention is encouraging at‐risk individuals to voluntarily and temporarily reduce access to firearms and other lethal methods. Yet delivering counseling on the topic can be difficult, given the political sensitivity of firearm discussions. To support such counseling, we sought to identify recommended framing and content of messages about reducing firearm access for suicide prevention. Through qualitative interviews with firearm owners and enthusiasts, we identified key points for use in framing (identity as a gun owner, trust, voluntary and temporary storage, and context and motivation) and specific content (preference for “firearm” over “gun,” and legal issues such as background checks for transfers). These findings build on prior work and should enhance efforts to develop and deliver effective, acceptable counseling and—ultimately—prevent firearm suicide. 相似文献