首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   669篇
  免费   13篇
  2021年   5篇
  2020年   6篇
  2019年   15篇
  2018年   19篇
  2017年   20篇
  2016年   12篇
  2015年   10篇
  2014年   14篇
  2013年   75篇
  2012年   21篇
  2011年   18篇
  2010年   20篇
  2009年   21篇
  2008年   28篇
  2007年   22篇
  2006年   18篇
  2005年   28篇
  2004年   24篇
  2003年   19篇
  2002年   22篇
  2001年   5篇
  2000年   10篇
  1999年   5篇
  1998年   18篇
  1997年   12篇
  1996年   10篇
  1995年   4篇
  1994年   9篇
  1993年   9篇
  1992年   8篇
  1991年   10篇
  1989年   7篇
  1988年   8篇
  1987年   8篇
  1984年   12篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   5篇
  1981年   9篇
  1980年   9篇
  1979年   8篇
  1978年   8篇
  1977年   12篇
  1976年   4篇
  1975年   4篇
  1974年   6篇
  1971年   5篇
  1970年   7篇
  1968年   6篇
  1967年   5篇
  1957年   4篇
排序方式: 共有682条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
31.
32.
33.
34.
Many studies indicate that increasing self-awareness leads to individuals reflecting on their values and ideals (Silvia & Duval, 2001). This self-reflection appears to increase prosocial behaviour (Berkowitz, 1987). However, previously studies typically manipulated self-awareness in situations in which the individual may have felt pressure from the researcher to help. Thus, experimenter pressure to behave prosocially confounds the self-awareness explanation provided in past research. We used a novel experimental paradigm to manipulate self-awareness and remove the researcher's presence to decrease the likelihood that the participant would conform to experimenter demand. Participants were 36 college students (Mage = 19.52; 25 women). The results indicated a strong probability that the experimental condition participants were more prosocial than control condition participants. These findings provide additional support for the hypothesis that self-reflection increases prosocial behaviour, even without experimenter demands. These findings and the importance of studying objective self-awareness in light of the coronavirus are discussed.  相似文献   
35.
36.
37.
This study investigated the types of self-reported main problems that persons report many years following a traumatic brain injury. This preliminary study is part of a large ongoing study of disability and handicap in adults following traumatic brain injury. As part of an extensive interview, subjects were asked an open-ended question regarding their current main problems which they thought resulted from their traumatic brain injury. Responses were obtained from 166 adult subjects (119 men and 47 women) whose time postinjury ranged from 9 to 24 years. Categories for responses were subsequently developed. The most commonly reported categories of problems were those relating to movement (39%), cognition (36%), and sensory impairment (31%). Findings suggest that subjects' long-term concerns were related to specific impairments more than to psychosocial, emotional, or behavioral issues. Methodological issues concerning this research are discussed in relation to findings.  相似文献   
38.
Is developing scientific thinking all about learning to control variables?   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Kuhn D  Dean D 《Psychological science》2005,16(11):866-870
Academically low-performing urban sixth graders engaged in inquiry activity received a suggestion that they focus their investigation on the role of a single factor. This suggestion had significant effects on their use of a superficially dissimilar strategy--controlling the variation of other factors. This latter strategy has received the lion's share of attention in research on the development of scientific reasoning. These results have implications, we propose, for what undergoes development with respect to scientific thinking and how this development can best be facilitated.  相似文献   
39.
40.
Contemporary cognitive models of obsessive-compulsive disorder emphasize the importance of various types of dysfunctional beliefs, such as beliefs about inflated responsibility, perfectionism and the importance of controlling one's thoughts. These beliefs have been conceptualized as main effects, each influencing obsessive-compulsive symptoms independent of the contributions of other beliefs. It is not known whether beliefs interact with one another in their influence on obsessive-compulsive symptoms. To investigate this issue, data from 248 obsessive-compulsive disorder patients were analyzed. Dependent variables were the factor scores on the 4 Padua Inventory subscales. Predictor variables were the factor scores from the 3 factors (inflated responsibility, perfectionism and controlling one's thoughts) of the Obsessive Beliefs Questionnaire and their 2- and 3-way interactions. Regression analyses revealed significant main effects; in almost all analyses one or more of inflated responsibility, perfectionism, and controlling one's thoughts factors predicted scores on the Padua factors even after controlling for general distress. There was no evidence that beliefs interact in their effects on obsessive-compulsive symptoms, thereby providing a relatively unusual instance in which a simpler explanation (main effects only) is just as powerful as a more complex model.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号