全文获取类型
收费全文 | 383篇 |
免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
384篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 15篇 |
2018年 | 16篇 |
2017年 | 13篇 |
2016年 | 23篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 14篇 |
2013年 | 32篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 19篇 |
2010年 | 12篇 |
2009年 | 10篇 |
2008年 | 18篇 |
2007年 | 21篇 |
2006年 | 15篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 20篇 |
2003年 | 15篇 |
2002年 | 14篇 |
2001年 | 6篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有384条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
Ana Morales Dawn C. Allain Patricia Arscott Emily James Gretchen MacCarrick Brittney Murray Crystal Tichnell Amy R. Shikany Sara Spencer Sara M. Fitzgerald-Butt Jessica D. Kushner Christi Munn Emily Smith Katherine G. Spoonamore Harikrishna S. Tandri W. Aaron Kay 《Journal of genetic counseling》2017,26(4):689-689
102.
To ascertain whether there are ear-hemisphere asymmetries of selective attention, signal stimuli (tonal sequences) were presented monaurally with and without complex maskers (music and speech). The right ear-left hemisphere was more disrupted by language maskers; the left ear-right hemisphere was more disrupted by music maskers. These results suggest that there are hemispheric asymmetries of selective attention and that the ear hemisphere that usually processes a class of stimuli has greater difficulty filtering out those stimuli than does the nonspecialized hemisphere. 相似文献
103.
The Immediate and Delayed Memory Tasks are variations of the Continuous Performance Test. While previous research with the tasks has focused on group performance across multiple blocks and sessions, reliability estimates have not been established. We estimated reliability (1) between blocks of a single testing session (N=106), (2) across four sessions within a single testing day (N=20), and (3) across sessions on four consecutive testing days (N=20). Analyses indicated that the primary variable of interest, i.e., commission of errors, showed acceptable reliability within and across testing periods. 相似文献
104.
Speed discrimination tasks were used to examine the spatial and temporal characteristics of the integration mechanism involved when signals are extended in the direction of motion. We varied the aspect ratio of a signal patch whose speed differed from the background, while holding the area of the signal patch constant, so that the signal patch could be either extended in the direction of motion or extended orthogonal to the direction of motion. Speed discrimination thresholds decreased dramatically as the signal patch was extended in the direction of motion. The spatial and temporal integration regions were larger than would be expected if the integration mechanism were a low-level motion detector. The mechanism was tuned for direction of motion. The data are discussed with reference to two alternative integration mechanisms: a low-level detector that is elongated in the direction of motion and a higher level integration mechanism characterized by cooperative or facilitatory interactions between low-level detectors tuned to the same direction of motion. Our data are consistent with a second-level, direction-specific process that integrates the responses of low-level motion detectors. 相似文献
105.
In this research, we propose that emotions are affected not only by appraisals, but also by a metacognitive sense of confidence versus doubt over the appraisals. Focusing on core-relational themes (CRTs), higher-order appraisals comprising the combined meaning of several appraisals, we predicted and found evidence, over two studies, that the effect of a CRT on the corresponding emotion is stronger if one feels confident about the validity of the CRT compared to feeling doubtful. In Study 1, CRT was manipulated by recall and in Study 2, CRT was manipulated in vivo. Both designs produced consistent support for the hypotheses. These findings demonstrate the need to consider metacognitive processes in understanding the effects of appraisals on emotions. 相似文献
106.
107.
108.
109.
Christopher M. Altman Dawn E. McQuiston Nadja Schreiber Compo 《Applied cognitive psychology》2019,33(3):426-438
Research shows that alcohol has a small and inconsistent effect on eyewitness recall and no effect on witnesses' lineup decisions. Much of this literature has tested participants with low‐to‐moderate blood alcohol concentration (BAC) levels, and no study has directly examined how identification procedure impacts intoxicated witnesses' decisions. In the present study, bar patrons' (N = 132) BAC levels were recorded before participating in a task. Midway through the task, they were interrupted by an intruder. Participants then recalled the incident via a staged interview and attempted to identify the intruder from a target‐present or target‐absent showup or lineup. Although elevated BAC levels (high as 0.24%) reduced the quantity and quality of information provided, BAC had no effect on witnesses' identification decisions regardless of format. Results highlight the importance of testing witness memory across a broad BAC spectrum and provide evidence that alcohol does not affect witnesses' identification ability. 相似文献
110.
Laura Mickes Travis M. Seale‐Carlisle Stacy A. Wetmore Scott D. Gronlund Steven E. Clark Curt A. Carlson Charles A. Goodsell Dawn Weatherford John T. Wixted 《Applied cognitive psychology》2017,31(5):467-477
From the perspective of signal detection theory, different lineup instructions may induce different levels of response bias. If so, then collecting correct and false identification rates across different instructional conditions will trace out the receiver operating characteristic (ROC)—the same ROC that, theoretically, could also be traced out from a single instruction condition in which each eyewitness decision is accompanied by a confidence rating. We tested whether the two approaches do in fact yield the same ROC. Participants were assigned to a confidence rating condition or to an instructional biasing condition (liberal, neutral, unbiased, or conservative). After watching a video of a mock crime, participants were presented with instructions followed by a six‐person simultaneous photo lineup. The ROCs from both methods were similar, but they were not exactly the same. These findings have potentially important policy implications for how the legal system should go about controlling eyewitness response bias.Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献