首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   141篇
  免费   94篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   3篇
  2017年   4篇
  2016年   4篇
  2014年   6篇
  2013年   7篇
  2012年   3篇
  2011年   8篇
  2010年   4篇
  2009年   4篇
  2008年   2篇
  2007年   6篇
  2006年   3篇
  2005年   4篇
  2004年   6篇
  2003年   6篇
  2002年   5篇
  2001年   8篇
  2000年   4篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   5篇
  1997年   5篇
  1996年   4篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   4篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   7篇
  1989年   5篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   5篇
  1985年   4篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   5篇
  1982年   5篇
  1980年   4篇
  1979年   6篇
  1978年   6篇
  1977年   4篇
  1976年   5篇
  1975年   4篇
  1974年   6篇
  1973年   7篇
  1972年   5篇
  1971年   3篇
  1970年   4篇
  1969年   4篇
  1968年   3篇
  1964年   1篇
排序方式: 共有235条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
191.
Three pigeons were trained on two-link chained fixed-interval fixed-interval schedules. Numbers of responses, time spent responding, and the total time spent in each component were measured. The data were analyzed according to the matching law for multiple and concurrent schedules. In most conditions, the ratio of response rates in the two links was a constant proportion of the ratio that would be predicted in a multiple schedule with the same components. Data on pauses during the interval schedules showed that, in most conditions, the pause duration was a linear function of the interval length, and greater in the initial link than in the terminal link. The experiment thus demonstrated a quantitative functional analysis of performance on a chained schedule.  相似文献   
192.
The Defining Issues Test (DIT) of moral reasoning development was administered to 20 advanced and 20 first-year graduate students, plus 40 college graduates matched to the graduate groups on sex, age, and verbal ability. A two-way ANOVA design was used to infer whether graduate education, selection, maturation, or a combination of these effects is (are) associated with group differences on the DIT. Results suggest that differences found between the graduate students and college graduates can be accounted for by a combination of maturational and selection effects, contrary to the hypothesized effects of formal education.  相似文献   
193.
Preference in concurrent variable-interval fixed-ratio schedules   总被引:10,自引:10,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
Five pigeons were trained on concurrent variable-interval fixed-ratio schedules in three experiments. Experiment 1 used two variable-interval schedules and one fixed-ratio schedule, and the ratio requirement was varied. Using the generalized matching law, sensitivity to reinforcement was close to 1.0, but performance was biased toward the variable-interval schedule with the lower reinforcement rate. In Experiment 2, which used one variable-interval and one fixed-ratio schedule, the interval schedule was varied. All birds showed sensitivities to reinforcement of less than 1.0 and of less than the values obtained in Experiment 1. The performance was also biased toward the fixed-ratio schedule. Because the generalized matching law could not account for the differences in the data from Experiments 1 and 2, an extension of this law was suggested and successfully tested in Experiment 3. The proposed dual-sensitivity model was also shown to clarify some previously reported results.  相似文献   
194.
Four experiments, each using the same six pigeons, investigated the effects of varying component durations and component reinforcement rates in multiple variable-interval schedules. Experiment 1 used unequal component durations in which one component was five times the duration of the other, and the shorter component was varied over conditions from 120 seconds to 5 seconds. The schedules were varied over five values for each pair of component durations. Sensitivity to reinforcement rate changes was the same at all component durations. In Experiment 2, both component durations were 5 seconds, and the schedules were again varied using both one and two response keys. Sensitivity to reinforcement was not different from the values found in Experiment 1. In Experiment 3, various manipulations, including body-weight changes, reinforcer duration changes, blackouts, hopper lights correlated with keylights, and overall reinforcement rate changes were carried out. No reliable increase in reinforcement sensitivity resulted from any manipulation. Finally, in Experiment 4, reinforcement rates in the two components were kept constant and unequal, and the component durations were varied. Shorter components produced significantly increased response rates normally in the higher reinforcement rate component, but schedule reversals at short component durations eliminated the response rate increases. The effects of component duration on multiple schedule performance cannot be interpreted as changing sensitivity to reinforcement nor to changing bias.  相似文献   
195.
196.
Choice: Some quantitative relations   总被引:13,自引:13,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
Six pigeons responded in fifty-six conditions on a concurrent-chains procedure. Conditions included several with equal initial links and unequal terminal links, several with unequal initial links and equal terminal links, and several with both unequal initial and terminal links. Although the delay-reduction hypothesis accounted well for choice when the initial links were equal (mean deviation of .04), it fit the data poorly when the initial links were unequal (mean deviation of .18). A modification of the delay-reduction hypothesis, replacing the rates of reinforcement with the square roots of these rates, fit the data better than either the unmodified delay-reduction equation or Killeen's (1982) model. The modified delay-reduction equation was also consistent with data from prior studies using concurrent chains. The absolute rates of responding in each terminal link were well described by the same hyperbola (Herrnstein, 1970) that describes response rates on simple interval schedules.  相似文献   
197.
Using data from magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), autopsy, endocranial measurements, and other techniques, we show that (1) brain size is correlated with cognitive ability about .44 using MRI; (2) brain size varies by age, sex, social class, and race; and (3) cognitive ability varies by age, sex, social class, and race. Brain size and cognitive ability show a curvilinear relation with age, increasing to young adulthood and then decreasing; increasing from women to men; increasing with socioeconomic status; and increasing from Africans to Europeans to Asians. Although only further research can determine if such correlations represent cause and effect, it is clear that the direction of the brain-size/cognitive-ability relationships described by Paul Broca (1824–1880), Francis Galton (1822–1911), and other nineteenth-century visionaries is true, and that the null hypothesis of no relation, strongly advocated over the last half century, is false.  相似文献   
198.
Leaving patches: An investigation of a laboratory analogue   总被引:2,自引:2,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
Five pigeons were trained on a procedure that has been used as a laboratory analogue to natural patch residence. Trials commenced with two responses available. One of these might provide a reinforcer if the patch was a prey patch; the other ended the residence time in the patch and, after a fixed travel time in blackout, produced another patch that might or might not provide a reinforcer. Patch residence also ended, and was followed by the same travel time, after a reinforcer was obtained or after a fixed maximum time was spent in the patch. The dependent variable was patch residence time, from the commencement of the patch to the time at which the subject emitted a response to exit from the patch or until the maximum patch residence time had elapsed. In Parts 1 to 3, the duration of the imposed travel time was varied from 0.25 to 16 s at three different probabilities (.05, .1, and .2) of food per second (λ) in prey patches. As reported in previous research, both increasing travel time and decreasing probabilities of reinforcers per second increased patch residence time. In Parts 4 to 7, the probability of prey trials (ρ) was varied in an irregular order from .1, through .2, .5, and .7, to .9 for different combinations of λ and travel time. Respectively, these were in Part 4, .05 per second and 0.25 s; in Part 5, .05 per second and 16 s; in Part 6, .2 per second and 0.25 s; and in Part 7, .2 per second and 16 s. A previously offered model, based on optimization assumptions, substantially and consistently underpredicted patch residence time. However, a modification of that model, which assumes that the subjects could not accurately discriminate the residence time that provided the minimum interreinforcer interval, described the data well. The same model also described previously reported residence times in a different species with a uniform distribution of prey-arrival times.  相似文献   
199.
With random assignment to treatments and standard assumptions, either a one-way ANOVA of post-test scores or a two-way, repeated measures ANOVA of pre- and post-test scores provides a legitimate test of the equal treatment effect null hypothesis for latent variable . In an ANCOVA for pre- and post-test variablesX andY which are ordinal measures of and , respectively, random assignment and standard assumptions ensure the legitimacy of inferences about the equality of treatment effects on latent variable . Sample estimates of adjustedY treatment means are ordinal estimators of adjusted post-test means on latent variable .  相似文献   
200.
The present study compared the performance of 6 pigeons trained to detect luminance differences in two different signal-detection procedures. Exposed to a three-key array, the pigeons were trained to peck the left key when the brighter of two light intensities had been presented on the center key and to peck the right key when the dimmer of two light intensities had been presented on the center key. Procedure A was a standard signal-detection procedure in which left/bright and right/dim responses produced food reinforcement and left/dim and right/bright responses produced periods of timeout. Procedure B was designed to simulate some of the contingencies operating in a prey-detection situation. Left-key responses produced reinforcement following the brighter center-key stimulus and a period of timeout following the dimmer center-key stimulus. Right-key responses always produced a short period of timeout irrespective of the stimulus. Within each procedure, the duration of timeout arranged for false alarms (left/dim responses) was varied between 3 s and 120 s. Measures of accuracy and response bias were compared between the two procedures. The timeout manipulation produced systematic, but relatively small, changes in these measures when right/dim responses (i.e., correct rejections) produced reinforcement (Procedure A). Arranging timeout for right/dim responses in Procedure B produced greater variability in accuracy and response bias than did arranging reinforcement, but this variability was not related to timeout duration. Overall, discrimination accuracy was considerably higher when right/dim responses produced timeout than when they resulted in reinforcement, and accuracy was accompanied by a large bias toward the response associated with reinforcement. These results are consistent with a recently proposed model of signal detection.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号