首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   76289篇
  免费   3153篇
  国内免费   29篇
  79471篇
  2020年   770篇
  2019年   952篇
  2018年   1344篇
  2017年   1334篇
  2016年   1439篇
  2015年   983篇
  2014年   1225篇
  2013年   5621篇
  2012年   2244篇
  2011年   2440篇
  2010年   1475篇
  2009年   1512篇
  2008年   2167篇
  2007年   2189篇
  2006年   2009篇
  2005年   1758篇
  2004年   1652篇
  2003年   1550篇
  2002年   1620篇
  2001年   2500篇
  2000年   2452篇
  1999年   1881篇
  1998年   877篇
  1997年   801篇
  1996年   772篇
  1993年   709篇
  1992年   1574篇
  1991年   1443篇
  1990年   1453篇
  1989年   1320篇
  1988年   1278篇
  1987年   1253篇
  1986年   1307篇
  1985年   1376篇
  1984年   1141篇
  1983年   1024篇
  1982年   748篇
  1981年   724篇
  1979年   1173篇
  1978年   875篇
  1975年   985篇
  1974年   1093篇
  1973年   1188篇
  1972年   979篇
  1971年   915篇
  1970年   802篇
  1969年   835篇
  1968年   1077篇
  1967年   957篇
  1966年   856篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
In this study we combined daily diary data with interview data to investigate individual differences in the impact of stressful daily events on mood. Using a sample of 96 women in an urban community, we examined perceived neighborhood quality and major life events as possibly potentiating the effects of stressful daily events, and we viewed social supports as potentially buffering this daily process. Results confirmed that the presence of chronic ecologic stress (neighborhood perceptions) exacerbated the immediate effects of stressful daily events on mood and also increased the likelihood of enduring effects of daily stressors on next day's mood. Contrary to expectations, previous exposure to major life events decreased the impact of stressful daily events. The availability of social supports, although not buffering the impact of stressful daily events on mood, did mitigate the enduring effects of these events on next day's mood. This study also presents a method for analyzing daily time-series data, while correcting for potential problems of autocorrelated error terms. As such, this study represents a significant advance over previous analytic approaches to time-series data in the study of the stress process.  相似文献   
992.
993.
994.
995.
Optimal timing and the Weber function   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
996.
A model for stimulus generalization in Pavlovian conditioning   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
  相似文献   
997.
998.
This is a survey of the prevalence of weight reducing and weight gaining in high school children. Our sample of 1,373 high school girls and boys was geographically, racially, and economically diverse. On the day of the survey, 63% of the girls and 16.2% of the boys reported being on weight reducing regimens; 9.1% of the girls and 28.4% of the boys were trying to gain weight. Most female reducers and male gainers were already normal weight. Compared to other racial groups, whites and Hispanics were more likely to be reducing, whereas blacks were more likely to be gaining. Exercise and moderate caloric reduction were most popular for weight reducing, and a small but significant number were regularly using fasting, vomiting, laxatives, and appetite suppressants. The direction of weight modification for girls and boys conformed to stereotyped physical ideals. The rate of weight reducing in female high school adolescents has increased significantly since similar surveys of American youths 20 years ago.  相似文献   
999.
Cognitive maps as orienting schemata   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
A "point-to-unseen-targets" task was used to test two theories about the nature of cognitive mapping. The hypothesis that a cognitive map is like a "picture in the head" predicts that (a) the cognitive map should have a preferred orientation and (b) all coded locations should be equally available. These predictions were confirmed in Experiments 1 and 3 when targets were cities in the northeastern United States and learning was from a map. The theory that a cognitive map is an orienting schema predicts that the cognitive map should have no preferred orientation and that targets in front of the body should be localized faster than targets behind the body. These predictions were confirmed in Experiments 1 and 2 when targets were local landmarks that had been learned via direct experience. In Experiment 3, when cities in the Northeast were targets and geographical knowledge had been acquired, in part, by traveling in the Northeast, the observed latency profiles were not as predicted by either theory of cognitive mapping. The results suggest that orienting schemata direct orientation with respect to local environments, but that orientation with respect to large geographical regions is supported by a different type of cognitive structure.  相似文献   
1000.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号