全文获取类型
收费全文 | 15291篇 |
免费 | 232篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
15525篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 88篇 |
2021年 | 135篇 |
2020年 | 247篇 |
2019年 | 287篇 |
2018年 | 389篇 |
2017年 | 389篇 |
2016年 | 415篇 |
2015年 | 291篇 |
2014年 | 374篇 |
2013年 | 1647篇 |
2012年 | 660篇 |
2011年 | 707篇 |
2010年 | 430篇 |
2009年 | 453篇 |
2008年 | 606篇 |
2007年 | 602篇 |
2006年 | 603篇 |
2005年 | 544篇 |
2004年 | 507篇 |
2003年 | 518篇 |
2002年 | 510篇 |
2001年 | 267篇 |
2000年 | 240篇 |
1999年 | 230篇 |
1998年 | 210篇 |
1997年 | 223篇 |
1996年 | 204篇 |
1995年 | 175篇 |
1994年 | 164篇 |
1993年 | 161篇 |
1992年 | 158篇 |
1991年 | 174篇 |
1990年 | 142篇 |
1989年 | 130篇 |
1988年 | 99篇 |
1987年 | 126篇 |
1986年 | 137篇 |
1985年 | 177篇 |
1984年 | 172篇 |
1983年 | 147篇 |
1982年 | 136篇 |
1981年 | 149篇 |
1980年 | 130篇 |
1979年 | 117篇 |
1978年 | 128篇 |
1977年 | 116篇 |
1976年 | 113篇 |
1975年 | 109篇 |
1974年 | 115篇 |
1973年 | 98篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
851.
852.
David Goldman 《Pacific Philosophical Quarterly》2014,95(1):1-22
In ‘Freedom and Resentment’ P. F. Strawson argues that reactive attitudes like resentment and indignation cannot be eliminated altogether, because doing so would involve exiting interpersonal relationships altogether. I describe an alternative to resentment: a form of moral sadness about wrongdoing that, I argue, preserves our participation in interpersonal relationships. Substituting this moral sadness for resentment and indignation would amount to a deep and far‐reaching change in the way we relate to each other – while keeping in place the interpersonal relationships, which, Strawson rightfully believes, cannot be eliminated. 相似文献
853.
854.
855.
David?Cruise?MalloyEmail author Ronald?Martin Thomas?Hadjistavropoulos Peilai?Liu Elizabeth?Fahey?McCarthy Ilhyeok?Park N?Shalani Masaaki?Murakami Suchat?Paholpak 《Philosophy, ethics, and humanities in medicine : PEHM》2014,9(1):18
Heidegger’s two modes of thinking, calculative and meditative, were used as the thematic basis for this qualitative study of physicians from seven countries (Canada, China, India, Ireland, Japan, Korea, & Thailand). Focus groups were conducted in each country with 69 physicians who cared for the elderly. Results suggest that physicians perceived ethical issues primarily through the lens of calculative thinking (76%) with emphasis on economic concerns. Meditative responses represented 24% of the statements and were mostly generated by Canadian physicians whose patients typically were not faced with economic barriers to treatment due to Canada’s universal health care system. 相似文献
856.
Vivian Dzokoto David S. Wallace Laura Peters Esi Bentsi-Enchill 《The Journal of general psychology》2014,141(2):151-168
In a modified replication of Strack, Martin, and Stepper's demonstration of the Facial Feedback Hypothesis (1988), we investigated the effect of attention to emotion on the facial feedback process in a non-western cultural setting. Participants, recruited from two universities in Ghana, West Africa, gave self-reports of their perceived levels of attention to emotion, and then completed cartoon-rating tasks while randomly assigned to smiling, frowning, or neutral conditions. While participants with low Attention to Emotion scores displayed the usual facial feedback effect (rating cartoons as funnier when in the smiling compared to the frowning condition), the effect was not present in individuals with high Attention to Emotion. The findings indicate that (1) the facial feedback process can occur in contexts beyond those in which the phenomenon has previously been studied, and (2) aspects of emotion regulation, such as Attention to Emotion can interfere with the facial feedback process. 相似文献
857.
858.
The architecture of personality 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Cervone D 《Psychological review》2004,111(1):183-204
This article presents a theoretical framework for analyzing psychological systems that contribute to the variability, consistency, and cross-situational coherence of personality functioning. In the proposed knowledge-and-appraisal personality architecture (KAPA), personality structures and processes are delineated by combining 2 principles: distinctions (a) between knowledge structures and appraisal processes and (b) among intentional cognitions with varying directions of fit, with the latter distinction differentiating among beliefs, evaluative standards, and aims. Basic principles of knowledge activation and use illuminate relations between knowledge and appraisal, yielding a synthetic account of personality structures and processes. Novel empirical data illustrate the heuristic value of the knowledge/appraisal distinction by showing how self-referent and situational knowledge combine to foster cross-situational coherence in appraisals of self-efficacy. 相似文献
859.
The current study examined whether stress reactivity becomes stronger or weaker with age. Daily stress and daily negative affect were modeled using 1,012 subjects from the National Study of Daily Events (NSDE), an 8-day daily diary study. Age ranged from 25 to 74. Data were modeled using within-person HLM techniques. Daily stress and neuroticism interacted in their effect on daily negative affect. There was a stronger association between daily stress and negative affect for persons high in neuroticism as compared to those low on the trait. In addition, daily stress and age interacted in their effect on daily negative affect. There was a stronger association between daily stress and negative affect for older as compared to younger adults. Results suggest heightened reactivity to stressors in older adulthood, perhaps due to kindling effects. Changes in the aging brain may explain this effect. Our investigations illuminate the complexities that characterize the set of associations among negative affect, stress, personality, and age, and point to potential aging or cohort effects. 相似文献
860.
The generality of working memory capacity: a latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning 总被引:12,自引:0,他引:12
Kane MJ Hambrick DZ Tuholski SW Wilhelm O Payne TW Engle RW 《Journal of experimental psychology. General》2004,133(2):189-217
A latent-variable study examined whether verbal and visuospatial working memory (WM) capacity measures reflect a primarily domain-general construct by testing 236 participants in 3 span tests each of verbal WM. visuospatial WM, verbal short-term memory (STM), and visuospatial STM. as well as in tests of verbal and spatial reasoning and general fluid intelligence (Gf). Confirmatory' factor analyses and structural equation models indicated that the WM tasks largely reflected a domain-general factor, whereas STM tasks, based on the same stimuli as the WM tasks, were much more domain specific. The WM construct was a strong predictor of Gf and a weaker predictor of domain-specific reasoning, and the reverse was true for the STM construct. The findings support a domain-general view of WM capacity, in which executive-attention processes drive the broad predictive utility of WM span measures, and domain-specific storage and rehearsal processes relate more strongly to domain-specific aspects of complex cognition. 相似文献